Dissertationen zum Thema „Working memory“
Geben Sie eine Quelle nach APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard und anderen Zitierweisen an
Machen Sie sich mit Top-50 Dissertationen für die Forschung zum Thema "Working memory" bekannt.
Neben jedem Werk im Literaturverzeichnis ist die Option "Zur Bibliographie hinzufügen" verfügbar. Nutzen Sie sie, wird Ihre bibliographische Angabe des gewählten Werkes nach der nötigen Zitierweise (APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver usw.) automatisch gestaltet.
Sie können auch den vollen Text der wissenschaftlichen Publikation im PDF-Format herunterladen und eine Online-Annotation der Arbeit lesen, wenn die relevanten Parameter in den Metadaten verfügbar sind.
Sehen Sie die Dissertationen für verschiedene Spezialgebieten durch und erstellen Sie Ihre Bibliographie auf korrekte Weise.
Morris, Neil Gerald. „Working memory constellations“. Thesis, Durham University, 1986. http://etheses.dur.ac.uk/7057/.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleTsukahara, Jason Seiichi. „The Role of Working Memory Resources in Mind Wandering: The Difference Between Working Memory Capacity and Working Memory Load“. CSUSB ScholarWorks, 2014. https://scholarworks.lib.csusb.edu/etd/81.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleJin, Ya-shyuan. „Is working memory working in consecutive interpreting?“ Thesis, University of Edinburgh, 2010. http://hdl.handle.net/1842/4451.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleDeFraine, William C. „Defining affective working memory“. Winston-Salem, NC : Wake Forest University, 2009. http://dspace.zsr.wfu.edu/jspui/handle/10339/42598.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleZokaei, N. „Modulation of working memory“. Thesis, University College London (University of London), 2013. http://discovery.ucl.ac.uk/1393276/.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleRalston, George Eastop. „Visuo-spatial working memory“. Thesis, University of St Andrews, 1988. http://hdl.handle.net/10023/9595.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleKlyn, Niall Andre Munson. „Working Memory for Rhythm“. The Ohio State University, 2012. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=osu1324305411.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleGlas, Brittany Michelle. „Working Memory and Aphasia“. Thesis, The University of Arizona, 2011. http://hdl.handle.net/10150/144356.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleTuthill, Frank. „The Effects of Working Memory Training and Encoding Strategy on Working Memory Capacity“. CSUSB ScholarWorks, 2018. https://scholarworks.lib.csusb.edu/etd/638.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleEastwood, Adrienne E. „Memory or attention?, understanding working memory in children“. Thesis, National Library of Canada = Bibliothèque nationale du Canada, 2001. http://www.collectionscanada.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk3/ftp05/NQ65235.pdf.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleIoannou, Panagiotis. „Computational modelling of working memory“. Thesis, University of Surrey, 2014. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.658622.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleSanderson, Katherine Elizabeth. „Working memory efficacy and aging“. Thesis, Northumbria University, 2006. http://nrl.northumbria.ac.uk/397/.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleRobson, Adam Jago. „Analogical reasoning and working memory“. Thesis, Durham University, 2012. http://etheses.dur.ac.uk/3625/.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleWallis, George J. „Selection mechanisms for working memory“. Thesis, University of Oxford, 2014. http://ora.ox.ac.uk/objects/uuid:1a9be8d6-9f8e-49af-8185-8be4c890010f.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleMok, Robert. „Working memory in healthy ageing“. Thesis, University of Oxford, 2016. https://ora.ox.ac.uk/objects/uuid:36594546-962e-4f81-ad91-589dcd96f029.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleKatz, David P. „The Fractionation of Working Memory“. Case Western Reserve University School of Graduate Studies / OhioLINK, 2019. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=case1559732086225506.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleGiofrè, David. „Geometry, working memory and intelligence“. Doctoral thesis, Università degli studi di Padova, 2013. http://hdl.handle.net/11577/3423374.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleLo studio della geometria è una parte fondamentale dell’apprendimento matematico ed ha una storia antica. Basti pensare come, ai tempi i cui Platone insegnava, l’ingresso nella scuola di Atene era proibito a coloro che non conoscevano la geometria. La geometria, inoltre, in epoca rinascimentale, faceva parte del ‘quadrivium’, ed era considerata uno studio necessario per intraprendere gli studi di filosofia. A dispetto dell’importanza che la geometria ha avuto nel passato, i processi cognitivi che sono alla base della geometria non sono ancora stati studiati in maniera dettagliata. Il presente lavoro di tesi si propone tre obiettivi. Primo, indagare la relazione tra vari aspetti della geometria e la memoria di lavoro visuospaziale (VSWM). Secondo, verificare se ragazzi con sindrome non verbale (NLD) presentino difficoltà in vari aspetti della geometria. Terzo, investigare la relazione tra vari aspetti della geometria, la memoria di lavoro (WM) e l’intelligenza (g). Nel secondo capitolo, viene fornita una panoramica sulla relazione tra geometria, WM e g. Dato che la geometria riguarda lo studio dello spazio, essa richiede un coinvolgimento attivo delle abilità spaziali. La WM, ed in particolare la VSWM, inoltre, sono coinvolte in maniera attiva in compiti geometrici. Risolvere problemi geometrici, in aggiunta, richiede di ragionare sul problema e trovare una soluzione tra le tante alternative possibili. Per questa ragione, l’intelligenza (g), è coinvolta in maniera attiva nella soluzione di problemi geometrici. Nel terzo capitolo, viene discussa la relazione tra geometria intuitiva (quella parte della geometria che sembra essere indipendente dalla cultura), geometria scolastica (la geometria che viene insegnata a scuola) e la VSWM. Vengono presentati due studi. Nel primo studio, è stata svolta una ricerca su 166 ragazzi frequentanti gli ultimi due anni della scuola secondaria di secondo grado. Lo studio prevedeva la presentazione di: 1) sei prove di VSWM, .2) una prova di geometria intuitiva (suddivisa in due parti: riguardanti principi core e mediati dalla cultura) 3) una prova di geometria scolastica. Dai risultati emerge come due prove di VSWM sono relate ad aspetti geometrici mediati dalla cultura i quali, insieme con principi ‘core’, che si pensa siano indipendenti dalla cultura, spiegano una porzione significativa di varianza delle prove di scolastica (14%). Nel secondo studio, la relazione tra VSWM e geometria (intuitiva e scolastica) è stata studiata considerando partecipanti con sintomi non verbali (NLD; i quali hanno problemi con prove spaziali, ma non con prove verbali). Lo studio ha preso in considerazione 16 partecipanti con NLD e 16 partecipanti appartenenti al gruppo di controllo. Dai risultati emerge come partecipanti con NLD cadano: i) in prove di geometria intuitiva (in aspetti ‘core’ e mediati dalla cultura), ii) in prove di geometria scolastica. partecipanti con NLD, inoltre, cadono anche in prove di VSWM. I risultati della ‘discriminant function analysis’, infine, confermano come prove di VSWM e geometriche siano importanti nel discriminare sintomi di NLD. Nel quarto capitolo, viene discussa la relazione tra geometria, memoria di lavoro e intelligenza . Nella prima parte dello studio viene analizzata la relazione tra WM e il fattore g. In un primo momento sono stati valutati diversi modelli di WM e il modello tripartito di Baddeley e Hitch (1974) è risultato essere quello che meglio si approssima ai dati (miglior fit). In un secondo momento, abbiamo analizzato la relazione tra il modello tripartito e il fattore g. L’analisi dimostra come due componenti della memoria di lavoro (memoria a breve termine verbale e memoria di lavoro) spighino una porzione consistente della varianza di g (65%). Nella seconda parte dello studio, vengono confrontati vari modelli concorrenti sulla relazione tra vari aspetti della geometria (intuitiva e scolastica), WM e g. Il modello con il migliore adattamento ai dati mostra come WM, con la mediazione del fattore g, spieghi una quota significativa di varianza della geometria scolastica e della geometria intuitiva. In aggiunta, i risultati dimostrano come una quota significativa di varianza sia condivisa tra il fattore generale e la geometria intuitiva. Nel quinto capitolo, viene presentata una panoramica generale degli studi presentati. Vengono, inoltre, evidenziati i limiti degli studi e i possibili sviluppi per studi futuri
Ivarsson, Magnus, und Stefan Strohmayer. „Working memory training improves arithmetic skills and verbal working memory capacity in children with ADHD“. Thesis, Stockholms universitet, Psykologiska institutionen, 2010. http://urn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:su:diva-49618.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleStone, James Michael. „How does practice affect working memory? : the efficacy of adaptive-difficulty working memory training programs“. Thesis, Lancaster University, 2015. http://eprints.lancs.ac.uk/85682/.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleRoome, Hannah. „The accessibility of memory items in children's working memory“. Thesis, Lancaster University, 2016. http://eprints.lancs.ac.uk/80979/.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleBraasch, Marie Y. „The Effects of Age and Working Memory Ability on Frontal Lobe Oxygenation During Working Memory Tasks“. Ohio University Honors Tutorial College / OhioLINK, 2010. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=ouhonors1275937579.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleGaunt, Joshua T. „Microsaccades and visual-spatial working memory /“. Diss., Digital Dissertations Database. Restricted to UC campuses, 2009. http://uclibs.org/PID/11984.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleWesenick, Maria-Barbara. „Limitations of Human Visual Working Memory“. Diss., lmu, 2004. http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:19-18677.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleVockenberg, Kerstin. „Updating of representations in working memory“. Phd thesis, kostenfrei, 2006. http://opus.kobv.de/ubp/volltexte/2007/1176/.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleWesterberg, Helena. „Working memory : development, disorders and training /“. Stockholm, 2004. http://diss.kib.ki.se/2004/91-7349-881-5/.
Der volle Inhalt der QuellePayne, Tabitha W. „Working memory capacity and pitch discrimination“. Diss., Georgia Institute of Technology, 2003. http://hdl.handle.net/1853/28831.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleWilsch, Anna. „Neural oscillations in auditory working memory“. Doctoral thesis, Universitätsbibliothek Leipzig, 2015. http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bsz:15-qucosa-177313.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleTerrence, Peter. „TACTILE WORKING MEMORY AND MULTIMODAL LOADING“. Doctoral diss., University of Central Florida, 2008. http://digital.library.ucf.edu/cdm/ref/collection/ETD/id/3584.
Der volle Inhalt der QuellePh.D.
Department of Psychology
Sciences
Psychology PhD
Holmes, Joni. „Working memory and children's mathematical skills“. Thesis, Durham University, 2005. http://etheses.dur.ac.uk/2205/.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleBrady, Timothy F. „Structured representations in visual working memory“. Thesis, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, 2011. http://hdl.handle.net/1721.1/68420.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleCataloged from PDF version of thesis.
Includes bibliographical references (p. 177-195).
How much visual information can we hold in mind at once? A large body of research has attempted to quantify the capacity of visual working memory by focusing on how many individual objects or visual features can be actively maintained in memory. This thesis presents a novel theoretical framework for understanding working memory capacity, suggesting that our memory representations are complex and structured even for simple visual displays, and formalizing such structured representations is necessary to understand the architecture and capacity of visual working memory. Chapter 1 reviews previous empirical research on visual working memory capacity, and argues that an understanding of memory capacity requires moving beyond quantifying how many items people can remember and instead focusing on the content of our memory representations. Chapter 2 argues for structured memory representations by demonstrating that we encode a summary of all of the items on a display in addition to information about particular items, and use both item and summary information to complete working memory tasks. Chapter 3 describes a computational model that formalizes the roles of perceptual organization and the encoding of summary statistics in visual working memory, and provides a way to quantify capacity even in the presence of richer, more structured memory representations. This formal framework predicts how well observers will be able to remember individual working memory displays, rather than focusing on average performance across many displays. Chapter 4 uses information theory to examine visual working memory through the framework of compression, and demonstrates that introducing regularities between items allows us to encode more colors in visual working memory. Thus, working memory capacity needs to be understood by taking into account learned knowledge, rather than simply focusing on the number of items to be remembered. Together, this research suggests that visual working memory capacity is best characterized by structured representations where prior knowledge influences how much can be stored and displays are encoded at multiple levels of abstraction.
by Timothy F. Brady.
Ph.D.
Möbius, Peter. „Working memory and second language comprehension“. Thesis, University of Cambridge, 2001. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.621230.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleGamsby, Christopher William. „Working Memory Updating using Meaningful Trigraphs“. Bowling Green State University / OhioLINK, 2016. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=bgsu1459339645.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleKniele, Kathryn Kloss Jacqueline D. „Emotional expressivity and working memory capacity /“. Philadelphia, Pa. : Drexel University, 2004. http://dspace.library.drexel.edu/handle/1860/399.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleSouza, Amarildo Lemes de. „Working memory and L2 vocabulary learning“. reponame:Repositório Institucional da UFSC, 2015. https://repositorio.ufsc.br/xmlui/handle/123456789/160546.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleMade available in DSpace on 2016-04-15T13:14:14Z (GMT). No. of bitstreams: 1 337911.pdf: 3308843 bytes, checksum: faac5426183647e8e4b2324f2b9d50a6 (MD5) Previous issue date: 2015
Abstract : Among the cognitive systems that underlie learning and use of a secondlanguage (L2), working memory emerges as one of the most studied andinfluential over the past 35 years (Dehn, 2011). Working memory is thesystem responsible for temporary storage and processing of informationduring the performance of complex activities such as languagecomprehension, learning and reasoning (Baddeley & Logie, 1999). Thepresent study investigates the influence of working memory capacity(Baddeley & Hitch, 1974) in L2 vocabulary learning in 24 younglearners aged 11-14 years attending the 6th grade of elementary school.The method used was quasi-experimental and mainly quantitative andcorrelational. The first stage of the study consisted of the assessment ofparticipants? working memory capacity through the Automated WorkingMemory Assessment - AWMA (Alloway, 2007) and assessment ofvocabulary knowledge through a pre-test in English as an L2. Thesecond stage of the study consisted of an intervention for explicitinstruction of 10 lexical items in the L2. The third and final stageconsisted of an immediate post-test to evaluate the retention of thelexical items which were the object of instruction. The results indicatedstatistically significant correlations between verbal working memory?sperformance vocabulary post-tests, showing that individuals withaverage or high performance on verbal working memory tests presentedbetter performance in recalling vocabulary words than individuals withlow verbal working memory performance. This suggests that verbalworking memory does in fact influence the acquisition of L2 vocabularyby young learners. These results are discussed in the light of Baddeley'sworking memory model (Baddeley, 2012).
Entre os sistemas cognitivos que fundamentam a aprendizagem e uso de uma língua segunda língua (L2), a memória de trabalho figura como um dos mais influentes e estudados dos últimos 35 anos (Dehn, 2011). Este é um sistema responsável pelo armazenamento e processamento temporário de informações durante a realização de atividades complexas tais como a compreensão da linguagem, a aprendizagem e o raciocínio (Baddeley & Logie, 1999). O presente estudo investiga a influência da capacidade de memória de trabalho (Baddeley & Hitch, 1974) na aprendizagem de vocabulário em segunda língua (L2) em 24 crianças na faixa etária de 11 a 14 anos, cursando o 6o ano do ensino fundamental. O método usado foi quase-experimental e predominantemente quantitativo e correlacional. A primeira etapa do estudo consistiu na avaliação da capacidade de memória de trabalho dos participantes por meio do Automated Working Memory Assessment  AWMA (Alloway, 2007) e de um pré-teste de vocabulário em inglês como L2. A segunda etapa do estudo consistiu de uma intervenção para instrução explícita de 10 itens lexicais na LE. A terceira e última etapa consistiu em um pósteste imediato para avaliação da retenção dos itens lexicais objeto de instrução. Os resultados obtidos indicaram uma correlação estatisticamente significativa entre o desempenho da memória de trabalho verbal e os pós-testes de vocabulário, indicando que indivíduos com melhor desempenho da memória de trabalho verbal apresentaram melhor desempenho na memorização de palavras do vocabulário do que indivíduos com baixo desempenho da memória de trabalho verbal. Isto sugere que a memória de trabalho verbal de fato influencia a aprendizagem de vocabulário em L2 por crianças. Estes resultados são discutidos à luz do modelo de memória de trabalho de Baddeley (BADDELEY, 2012).
Fried, Peter Jacob. „Functional asymmetries in human working memory“. Thesis, Boston University, 2013. https://hdl.handle.net/2144/10997.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleWorking memory is the cognitive ability to maintain and manipulate information in mind to guide behavior. This relies on the coordinated activity of a bilateral brain network, which has been modeled as a central executive in control of separate storage systems for verbal and spatial information. Evidence from human and nonhuman primate research demonstrates that the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) is critical for manipulating information in working memory. However, whether the dlPFC is dissociable by the domain of information remains unsettled. Recent human studies using repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) suggest the left and right dlPFC may play separable roles in manipulating verbal and spatial information. In the present study, this theory was investigated further with two experiments on healthy right-handed adults. Both experiments utilized the 3-back task of visual working memory with letters and locations serving as verbal and spatial stimuli, respectively. In Experiment 1, tasks were administered during functional neuroimaging in two formats: one using centrally-presented single letters as verbal stimuli, and dots in different locations as spatial stimuli; and another using single letters in different locations for both verbal and spatial tasks. At the whole-brain group-level, letter- and location-specific contrasts did not differ between formats, indicating verbal/spatial differences reflected discrete subsystems in working memory and not simply separate perceptual processing. Nevertheless, in the dlPFC, bilateral activity was observed across versions, suggesting its contributions to working memory are domain-independent. Experiment 2 tested whether this relationship was causal by assessing 3-back performance after applying low-frequency rTMS to the dlPFC. Following rTMS of the right dlPFC, accuracy improved on the letter task, but worsened on the location task, while the opposite was observed after left rTMS. These double-dissociations suggest left and right dlPFC operate as competing subsystems for manipulating verbal and spatial information, respectively. Thus, the observation of equivalent bilateral dlPFC activity during the letter and location tasks might reflect a left-lateralized system for verbally-encoded information and a right-lateralized system for nonverbal representations operating in parallel on all stimuli. Such a functional asymmetry would have implications for therapies aimed at ameliorating working memory impairments in disease and even normal aging.
Threadgold, Emma. „The relationship between processing and memory in working memory development“. Thesis, Lancaster University, 2011. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.656860.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleMorey, Candice Coker. „Maintaining cross-domain objects and features in working memory implications for storage in models of working memory /“. Diss., Columbia, Mo. : University of Missouri-Columbia, 2007. http://hdl.handle.net/10355/4889.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleThe entire dissertation/thesis text is included in the research.pdf file; the official abstract appears in the short.pdf file (which also appears in the research.pdf); a non-technical general description, or public abstract, appears in the public.pdf file. Title from title screen of research.pdf file (viewed on February 26, 2008) Vita. Includes bibliographical references.
Carter, Carole. „Children's working memory and academic achievement : the relationship between visuo-spatial working memory and progress in arithmetic“. Thesis, University of Sunderland, 2002. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.393052.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleSevey, Brittany Christine. „Mathematics anxiety, working memory, and mathematics performance: Effectiveness of a working memory intervention on reducing mathematics anxiety“. Miami University / OhioLINK, 2012. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=miami1302371469.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleSmith-Spark, James Hugo. „Memory in adult dyslexics : an exploration of the working memory system“. Thesis, University of Sheffield, 2000. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.434725.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleClick, Ivy A., S. K. Thacker, BK S. Beale, G. D. Frye und Russell W. Brown. „Mecamylamine Blocks Nicotine’s Enhancement of Reference Memory but Not Working Memory“. Digital Commons @ East Tennessee State University, 2002. https://dc.etsu.edu/etsu-works/6423.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleSahu, Aparna A. „The Roles of Individual Differences and Working Memory in Episodic Memory“. University of Toledo / OhioLINK, 2013. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=toledo1365166387.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleNiese, Adam Trent. „The Relationship between visual working memory and visual long-term memory“. Diss., University of Iowa, 2008. https://ir.uiowa.edu/etd/210.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleSmith, Lauren M. „Rumination, negative affect and working memory| Does rumination moderate the relationship between negative affect induction and working memory?“ Thesis, Seattle Pacific University, 2013. http://pqdtopen.proquest.com/#viewpdf?dispub=3593597.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleDepression affects about 20% of the U.S. population at some point in their lifespan (Gotlib & Hammen, 2002). One symptom of depression is impairment in cognitive functioning. Extensive research has previously identified a link between depressed mood and memory difficulties (Burt, Zembar, & Niederehe, 1995; O'Conner, Pollitt, Roth, Brook, & Reiss, 1990; Watkins & Teasdale, 2004). The purpose of the current study is to better understand the relationship between negative affect and memory impairment. I hypothesized that rumination would moderate the relationship between negative affect and working memory such that individuals who respond to negative affect with rumination would be particularly likely to show impairment in working memory. This was a single time point study in which participants were randomly assigned to one of two possible conditions. In each condition, participants were given a stressor task, the Paced Auditory Serial Addition Task (PASAT). This was followed by either failure feedback or success feedback. 146 undergraduate students, ages 18 to 30 were recruited and randomly assigned to one of the two conditions. The sample was approximately 79% female and 78% Caucasian and had a mean age of 18.77 (SD = 1.36). Participants completed measures of current depressive symptoms, trait rumination, affective state pre and post stressor task, and working memory. This study's findings lend support to previous research in that these results yielded a significant main effect of both the failure condition (F (1, 143) = 124.20, p = .00, partial &eegr; 2 = .47) and self-reported negative mood (F (3, 145) = 14.59, p = .00, R2 = .22) on lower working memory scores. Greater rumination appeared to have a main effect of lower working memory scores (F (2, 139) = 12.59, p = .00, partial &eegr;2 = .15) with rumination accounting for approximately 4% of the difference in working memory scores. However, results did not find support for a moderated model (F (2, 139) = .02, p = .98, partial &eegr;2 = .00). Although negative affect and rumination predicted working memory scores, rumination did not moderate the relationship suggesting that a different model may explain the cognitive effects of depression.
Pertab, Jon Leroy. „The Neural Systems of Working Memory: The Sternberg Working Memory Task in a Pediatric Traumatic Brain Injury Sample“. BYU ScholarsArchive, 2010. https://scholarsarchive.byu.edu/etd/2520.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleHarrison, Tyler Leland. „The role of working memory capacity and fluid intelligence in the organization of memory“. Thesis, Georgia Institute of Technology, 2014. http://hdl.handle.net/1853/53115.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleLin, Mingkuan. „Genetic association study of spatial working memory“. Fairfax, VA : George Mason University, 2009. http://hdl.handle.net/1920/4591.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleVita: p. 120. Thesis director: Karl J. Fryxell. Submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Bioinformatics. Title from PDF t.p. (viewed Oct. 12, 2009). Includes bibliographical references (p. 105-119). Also issued in print.
Karakuyu, Dilek. „Dopaminergic and serotonergic modulation of working memory“. [S.l.] : [s.n.], 2003. http://deposit.ddb.de/cgi-bin/dokserv?idn=970180993.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleBarbosa, João Moura. „Neural network mechanisms of working memory interference“. Doctoral thesis, Universitat de Barcelona, 2019. http://hdl.handle.net/10803/669202.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleGozenman, Filiz. „Interaction Of Probability Learning And Working Memory“. Master's thesis, METU, 2012. http://etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12614535/index.pdf.
Der volle Inhalt der Quelle