Zeitschriftenartikel zum Thema „Visual perception“

Um die anderen Arten von Veröffentlichungen zu diesem Thema anzuzeigen, folgen Sie diesem Link: Visual perception.

Geben Sie eine Quelle nach APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard und anderen Zitierweisen an

Wählen Sie eine Art der Quelle aus:

Machen Sie sich mit Top-50 Zeitschriftenartikel für die Forschung zum Thema "Visual perception" bekannt.

Neben jedem Werk im Literaturverzeichnis ist die Option "Zur Bibliographie hinzufügen" verfügbar. Nutzen Sie sie, wird Ihre bibliographische Angabe des gewählten Werkes nach der nötigen Zitierweise (APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver usw.) automatisch gestaltet.

Sie können auch den vollen Text der wissenschaftlichen Publikation im PDF-Format herunterladen und eine Online-Annotation der Arbeit lesen, wenn die relevanten Parameter in den Metadaten verfügbar sind.

Sehen Sie die Zeitschriftenartikel für verschiedene Spezialgebieten durch und erstellen Sie Ihre Bibliographie auf korrekte Weise.

1

Atasoy, Seda Nur. „Visual perception management in advertisements“. New Trends and Issues Proceedings on Humanities and Social Sciences 2, Nr. 1 (19.02.2016): 397–409. http://dx.doi.org/10.18844/gjhss.v2i1.324.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
2

Cooper, John, Nicholas J. Wade und Michael Swanston. „Visual Perception“. Leonardo 24, Nr. 5 (1991): 631. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1575684.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
3

Ng, Jason S. „Visual Perception“. Optometry and Vision Science 95, Nr. 12 (Dezember 2018): 1166–67. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/opx.0000000000001332.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
4

KEARINS, JUDITH. „Visual Perception“. Australian Occupational Therapy Journal 14, Nr. 1 (27.08.2010): 5–20. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1440-1630.1967.tb00189.x.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
5

Boselie, F. „Visual perception“. Acta Psychologica 75, Nr. 2 (November 1990): 174–75. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0001-6918(90)90088-w.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
6

Sahan, Melek. „On three dimensional perceptions in the visual age“. Global Journal of Arts Education 5, Nr. 1 (14.04.2015): 10. http://dx.doi.org/10.18844/gjae.v5i1.14.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
Annotation:
Aim of the study is to examine the values of the three dimensional perception and collect information about that. Starting point is the observation of differences in two and three dimensional perception abilities of individuals having education in visual arts. It has been observed that some students, who have high level of competency in two dimensional perception, do not have the same competency in three dimensional perception. This is considered important since it creates two and three dimensional perceptions and differences of expression between them. We tried to emphasize the importance of the subject particularly in terms of visual arts education in the current visual age. The study is a document review. In this study we addressed the characteristics of the age we are living in, perception, visual perception, and two and three dimensional perception. We summarized the differences in visual perception and included three dimensional comprehension. By examining the related literature, we came to the conclusion that the two and three dimensional perception processes and their requirements are different. Keywords: sculpture, three dimensional perceptions, visual perception, visual age.
7

Taneja, MK. „Visual speech perception“. Indian Journal of Otology 25, Nr. 2 (2019): 49. http://dx.doi.org/10.4103/indianjotol.indianjotol_67_19.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
8

McSorley, Eugene. „Review: Visual Perception“. Perception 30, Nr. 4 (April 2001): 523–24. http://dx.doi.org/10.1068/p3004rvw.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
9

Klingenhoefer, Steffen, und Bart Krekelberg. „Perisaccadic visual perception“. Journal of Vision 17, Nr. 9 (24.08.2017): 16. http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/17.9.16.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
10

Albright, T. D., und G. R. Stoner. „Visual motion perception.“ Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 92, Nr. 7 (28.03.1995): 2433–40. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.92.7.2433.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
11

Mascalzoni, Elena, und Lucia Regolin. „Animal visual perception“. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science 2, Nr. 1 (20.08.2010): 106–16. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/wcs.97.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
12

Revonsuo, Antti. „Visual perception and subjective visual awareness“. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21, Nr. 6 (Dezember 1998): 769–70. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x98461752.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
Annotation:
Pessoa et al. fail to make a clear distinction between visual perception and subjective visual awareness. Their most controversial claims, however, concern subjective visual awareness rather than visual perception: visual awareness is externalized to the “personal level,” thus denying the view that consciousness is a natural biological phenomenon somehow constructed inside the brain.
13

Hardcastle, Valerie Gray. „Visual perception is not visual awareness“. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24, Nr. 5 (Oktober 2001): 985. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x01360119.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
Annotation:
O'Regan & Noë mistakenly identify visual processing with visual experience. I outline some reasons why this is a mistake, taking my data and arguments mainly from the literature on subliminal processing.
14

Torales, Julio, Enrique De Doménico Meyer und Eliana Duarte Fariña. „Schizophrenia and visual perception disturbances“. Revista Virtual de la Sociedad Paraguaya de Medicina Interna 7, Nr. 2 (30.09.2020): 96–104. http://dx.doi.org/10.18004/rvspmi/2312-3893/2020.07.02.96.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
15

Teraoka, Ryo, und Wataru Teramoto. „Touch-contingent visual motion perception: tactile events drive visual motion perception“. Experimental Brain Research 235, Nr. 3 (03.12.2016): 903–12. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-016-4850-y.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
16

Legrain, Valéry, Louise Manfron, Marynn Garcia und Lieve Filbrich. „Does Body Perception Shape Visuospatial Perception?“ Perception 47, Nr. 5 (15.03.2018): 507–20. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0301006618763269.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
Annotation:
How we perceive our body is shaped by sensory experiences with our surrounding environment, as witnessed by poor performance in tasks during which participants judge with their hands crossed the temporal order between two somatosensory stimuli, one applied on each hand. This suggests that somatosensory stimuli are not only processed according to a somatotopic representation but also a spatiotopic representation of the body. We investigated whether the perception of stimuli occurring in external space, such as visual stimuli, can also be influenced by the body posture and somatosensory stimuli. Participants performed temporal order judgements on pairs of visual stimuli, one in each side of space, with their hands uncrossed or crossed. In Experiment 1, participants’ hands were placed either near or far from the visual stimuli. In Experiment 2, the visual stimuli were preceded, either by 60 ms or 360 ms, by tactile stimuli applied on the hands placed near the visual stimuli. Manipulating the time interval was intended to activate either a somatotopic or a spatiotopic representation of somatic inputs. We did not obtain any evidence for an influence of body posture on visual temporal order judgment, suggesting that body perception is less relevant for processing extrabody stimuli than the reverse.
17

Cuijpers, R. H., A. M. L. Kappers und J. J. Koenderink. „Visual perception of collinearity“. Perception & Psychophysics 64, Nr. 3 (April 2002): 392–404. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/bf03194712.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
18

Duchesneau, André P. „Alexithymia and Visual Perception“. Perceptual and Motor Skills 83, Nr. 1 (August 1996): 291–98. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pms.1996.83.1.291.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
Annotation:
60 subjects were administered the Toronto Alexithymia Scale-20, a screening test for alexithymia, and categorized by the usual scoring procedure. Each subject was then given the six Performance subtests of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised which address aspects of visual perception, organization, and construction. Analysis indicated that the alexithymic group scored significantly lower on two tests of visual perception, Picture Completion and Digit Symbol of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Revised.
19

Ittelson, William H. „Visual perception of markings“. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 3, Nr. 2 (Juni 1996): 171–87. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/bf03212416.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
20

Heider, Daniel. „Suárez on Visual Perception“. Scientia et Fides 5, Nr. 1 (19.04.2017): 61. http://dx.doi.org/10.12775/setf.2017.003.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
21

NAGAMACHI, Mibuo. „Visual Perception and Visualization.“ Journal of the Visualization Society of Japan 11, Nr. 43 (1991): 201–7. http://dx.doi.org/10.3154/jvs.11.201.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
22

Zahorik, Pavel. „Auditory/visual distance perception“. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 137, Nr. 4 (April 2015): 2374. http://dx.doi.org/10.1121/1.4920626.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
23

Stryker, Michael P. „Elements of visual perception“. Nature 360, Nr. 6402 (November 1992): 301–2. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/360301a0.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
24

Mirabel, Sylvie. „Visual function and perception“. Current Opinion in Ophthalmology 3, Nr. 5 (Oktober 1992): 581–91. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00055735-199210000-00005.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
25

Schomaker, Judith, und Martijn Meeter. „Novelty Enhances Visual Perception“. PLoS ONE 7, Nr. 12 (05.12.2012): e50599. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0050599.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
26

Zhang, J., und S. Y. Wu. „Structure of visual perception.“ Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 87, Nr. 20 (01.10.1990): 7819–23. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.87.20.7819.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
27

Brown, Angela M. „Visual Perception: Essential Readings.“ Optometry and Vision Science 79, Nr. 5 (Mai 2002): 283. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00006324-200205000-00005.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
28

Celesia, Gastone G. „Visual Perception and Awareness“. Journal of Psychophysiology 24, Nr. 2 (Januar 2010): 62–67. http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/0269-8803/a000014.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
Annotation:
The study of visual processing and abnormalities due to lesions of cortical structures sheds light on visual awareness/consciousness and may help us to better understand consciousness. We report on clinical observations and psychophysical testing of achromatopsia/prosopagnosia, visual agnosia, and blindsight. Achromatopsia and prosopagnosia reveal that visual cortices have functionally specialized processing systems for color, face perception, and their awareness, and that furthermore these systems operate independently. Dysfunction is limited to some aspects of visual perception; someone with achromatopsia, although not conscious of color, is aware of the objects’ form, motion, and their relationship with sound and other sensory percepts. Perceptual awareness is modular, with neuronal correlates represented by multiple separate specialized structures or modules. Visual agnosia shows that awareness of a complete visual percept is absent, though the subject is aware of single visual features such as edges, motion, etc., an indication that visual agnosia is a disruption of the binding process that unifies all information into a whole percept. Blindsight is characterized by the subject’s ability to localize a visual target while denying actually seeing the target. Blindsight is mediated by residual islands of the visual cortex, which suggests that sensory modules responsible for awareness can function only when structurally intact. We conclude (1) that perceptual awareness (consciousness?) is modular, and (2) that perceptual integration is also modular, which suggests that integration among distinct cortical regions is a parallel process with multiple communication pathways. Any hypothesis about consciousness must include these observations about the presence of multiple parallel, but spatially and temporally different, mechanisms.
29

Beauchamp, Ross. „THEORIES OF VISUAL PERCEPTION“. Optometry and Vision Science 68, Nr. 9 (September 1991): 758. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00006324-199109000-00019.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
30

Griffin, John R., Todd F. Birch, Gordon F. Bateman und Paul N. De Land. „Dyslexia and Visual Perception“. Optometry and Vision Science 70, Nr. 5 (Mai 1993): 374–79. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00006324-199305000-00006.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
31

Jolij, Jacob, und Maaike Meurs. „Music Alters Visual Perception“. PLoS ONE 6, Nr. 4 (21.04.2011): e18861. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0018861.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
32

Stuchlíková, Iva, Pavel Kindlman und Frantisek Man. „Anxiety in visual perception“. Anxiety, Stress & Coping 9, Nr. 1 (Januar 1996): 87–103. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10615809608249394.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
33

Troscianko, Tom, Christopher P. Benton, P. George Lovell, David J. Tolhurst und Zygmunt Pizlo. „Camouflage and visual perception“. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 364, Nr. 1516 (06.11.2008): 449–61. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2008.0218.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
Annotation:
How does an animal conceal itself from visual detection by other animals? This review paper seeks to identify general principles that may apply in this broad area. It considers mechanisms of visual encoding, of grouping and object encoding, and of search. In most cases, the evidence base comes from studies of humans or species whose vision approximates to that of humans. The effort is hampered by a relatively sparse literature on visual function in natural environments and with complex foraging tasks. However, some general constraints emerge as being potentially powerful principles in understanding concealment—a ‘constraint’ here means a set of simplifying assumptions. Strategies that disrupt the unambiguous encoding of discontinuities of intensity (edges), and of other key visual attributes, such as motion, are key here. Similar strategies may also defeat grouping and object-encoding mechanisms. Finally, the paper considers how we may understand the processes of search for complex targets in complex scenes. The aim is to provide a number of pointers towards issues, which may be of assistance in understanding camouflage and concealment, particularly with reference to how visual systems can detect the shape of complex, concealed objects.
34

Boynton, Geoffrey M. „Attention and visual perception“. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 15, Nr. 4 (August 2005): 465–69. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2005.06.009.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
35

Hartmann, Joel A., William A. Wolz, David P. Roeltgen und Felice L. Loverso. „Denial of visual perception“. Brain and Cognition 16, Nr. 1 (Mai 1991): 29–40. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0278-2626(91)90083-k.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
36

Slater, Alan M. „Visual perception at birth“. Infant Behavior and Development 9 (April 1986): 346. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0163-6383(86)80352-6.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
37

Hel, Francois. „Visual Perception Entropy Measure“. SMPTE Motion Imaging Journal 128, Nr. 10 (November 2019): 51–57. http://dx.doi.org/10.5594/jmi.2019.2941361.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
38

Zakia, Richard D. „Photography and Visual Perception“. Journal of Aesthetic Education 27, Nr. 4 (1993): 67. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/3333501.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
39

Langley, Keith. „Visual perception and encoding“. Spatial Vision 18, Nr. 4 (2005): 375–77. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/1568568054389598.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
40

Matthews, Julia. „Neurobiology of Visual Perception“. Psychoanalytic Quarterly 63, Nr. 2 (April 1994): 392–403. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/21674086.1994.11927419.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
41

Nakayama, K. „Binocular visual surface perception.“ Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 93, Nr. 2 (23.01.1996): 634–39. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.93.2.634.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
42

Faubert, Jocelyn. „Visual perception and aging.“ Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie expérimentale 56, Nr. 3 (2002): 164–76. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0087394.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
43

ffytche, D. H., J. D. Blom und M. Catani. „Disorders of visual perception“. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry 81, Nr. 11 (22.10.2010): 1280–87. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.2008.171348.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
44

Doi, S. A. R. „Visual perception — A hypothesis“. Medical Hypotheses 35, Nr. 2 (Juni 1991): 148–50. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0306-9877(91)90039-2.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
45

Vos, P. G. „Theories of visual perception“. Acta Psychologica 75, Nr. 1 (Oktober 1990): 91–92. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0001-6918(90)90068-q.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
46

Wagemans, Johan. „Visual perception: An introduction“. Acta Psychologica 81, Nr. 1 (Oktober 1992): 91–93. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0001-6918(92)90016-7.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
47

Johnson, Scott P. „Development of visual perception“. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science 2, Nr. 5 (22.11.2010): 515–28. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/wcs.128.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
48

Intraub, Helene. „Rethinking visual scene perception“. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science 3, Nr. 1 (06.05.2011): 117–27. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/wcs.149.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
49

Porto, André. „SINGULARITY AND VISUAL PERCEPTION“. Revista Dissertatio de Filosofia 58 (13.03.2024): 218–46. http://dx.doi.org/10.15210/dissertatio.v58i.26546.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
Annotation:
This paper deals with the mutations in Wittgenstein’s treatment of the notions of “generality”and of “singularity”, from his first philosophy, in the Tractatus Logico-Philosophicus, to his later maturephilosophy represented by the Philosophical Investigations. As we shall see, Wittgenstein’s philosophicalhandling of the notion of “visual perception” plays a key role in those conceptual transformations.
50

Ishai, Alumit, und Dov Sagi. „Visual Imagery Facilitates Visual Perception: Psychophysical Evidence“. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 9, Nr. 4 (Juli 1997): 476–89. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn.1997.9.4.476.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
Annotation:
Visual imagery is the invention or recreation of a perceptual experience in the absence of retinal input.The degree to which the same neural representations are involved in both visual imagery and visual perception is unclear. Previous studies have shown that visual imagery interferes with perception (Perky effect). We report here psychophysical data showing a direct facilitatory effect of visual imagery on visual perception. Using a lateral masking detection paradigm of a Gabor target, flanked by peripheral Gabor masks, observers performed imagery tasks that were preceded by perceptual tasks. We found that both perceived and imaginary flanking masks can reduce contrast detection threshold. At short target-to-mask distances imagery induced a threshold reduction of 50% as compared with perception, while at long target-to-mask distances imagery and perception had similar facilitatory effect. The imagery-induced facilitation was specific to the orientation of the stimulus, as well as to the eye used in the task. These data indicate the existence of a stimulus-specific short-term memory system that stores the sensory trace and enables reactivation of quasi-pictorial representations by topdown processes. We suggest that stimulus parameters dominate the imagery-induced facilitation at short target-to-mask distances, yet the topdown component contributes to the effect at long target-to-mask distances.

Zur Bibliographie