Dissertationen zum Thema „Theory of mind“
Geben Sie eine Quelle nach APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard und anderen Zitierweisen an
Machen Sie sich mit Top-50 Dissertationen für die Forschung zum Thema "Theory of mind" bekannt.
Neben jedem Werk im Literaturverzeichnis ist die Option "Zur Bibliographie hinzufügen" verfügbar. Nutzen Sie sie, wird Ihre bibliographische Angabe des gewählten Werkes nach der nötigen Zitierweise (APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver usw.) automatisch gestaltet.
Sie können auch den vollen Text der wissenschaftlichen Publikation im PDF-Format herunterladen und eine Online-Annotation der Arbeit lesen, wenn die relevanten Parameter in den Metadaten verfügbar sind.
Sehen Sie die Dissertationen für verschiedene Spezialgebieten durch und erstellen Sie Ihre Bibliographie auf korrekte Weise.
Cussins, A. „A representational theory of mind“. Thesis, University of Oxford, 1986. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.375861.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleO'Connell, Sanjida. „Theory of mind in chimpanzees“. Thesis, University of Liverpool, 1996. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.321147.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleNicolls, Small Lucinda. „Descartes’ Developmental Theory of Mind“. Thesis, The University of Sydney, 2022. https://hdl.handle.net/2123/29781.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleKnoll, Meredith Sharyn. „Rethinking the #theory' in theory of mind development“. Thesis, University College London (University of London), 2001. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.272550.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleWalters, Daniel Dewi. „Understanding other minds : an interrogation of the theory of mind debate“. Thesis, University of Hull, 2014. http://hydra.hull.ac.uk/resources/hull:11523.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleVestberg, Marcus Erik. „A compatibilist computational theory of mind“. Thesis, Oxford Brookes University, 2017. https://radar.brookes.ac.uk/radar/items/bf378e8f-abc3-47cd-9004-4efab56970c1/1/.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleMeyer, Joseph Francis. „Theory of Mind Impairment and Schizotypy“. W&M ScholarWorks, 2005. https://scholarworks.wm.edu/etd/1539626502.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleSharp, Carla. „Biased minds : theory of mind in emotional behaviour disorders of middle childhood“. Thesis, University of Cambridge, 2000. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.621991.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleHwang, Yoon Suk. „Mind and autism spectrum disorders: A Theory-of-Mind continuum model and typology developed from Theory-of Mind as subjectively experienced and objectively understood“. Thesis, The University of Sydney, 2009. http://hdl.handle.net/2123/5984.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleHwang, Yoon Suk. „Mind and autism spectrum disorders: A Theory-of-Mind continuum model and typology developed from Theory-of Mind as subjectively experienced and objectively understood“. University of Sydney, 2009. http://hdl.handle.net/2123/5984.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleThis study defines Theory-of-Mind as the ability to experience one’s own mind and understand the minds of others to the extent necessary to make sense of human behaviour and the world. Since the concept of Theory-of-Mind was first applied to people with ASD (Baron-Cohen, Leslie & Frith, 1985), lack of Theory-of-Mind has been used to explain their cognitive difficulties (National Research Council, 2003), along with social, communicative and imaginative impairments (Frith, Happé & Siddons, 1994). Previous studies have tended to think of Theory-of-Mind in terms of a simple binary of deficit or credit; to exclude the voices of people with ASD; to emphasise the cognitive aspects of Theory-of-Mind over its affective aspects; and to emphasise understanding the minds of others over experiencing one’s own mind. This study aims to address these issues by investigating Theory-of-Mind as subjectively experienced by students with ASD and objectively understood by their teachers. It is the first attempt in the study of Theory-of-Mind to include the voices of individuals with ASD along with the professional views of their teachers. This study takes an interdisciplinary approach, supported by philosophy of mind and special education. A grounded theory approach and a mixed methods research design combine to build and strengthen a theory of Theory-of-Mind. For Theory-of-Mind as subjectively experienced, 20 senior secondary and post secondary school students with ASD from Republic of Korea were interviewed and student-produced documents were reviewed to draw out their inner experiences. The Korean Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale and the Korean Vineland Social Maturity Scale were employed to assess IQ and social competence. For Theory-of-Mind as objectively understood, their teachers’ beliefs regarding their students with ASD were sought through in-depth interviews, a review of teacher-produced documents and administration of a newly developed Teacher Questionnaire. This study reports differences between Theory-of-Mind as subjectively experienced and objectively observed, and variations within the components of Theory-of-Mind. The role of imagination in Theory-of-Mind and the relationships between Theory-of-Mind components, IQ and social competence are discussed. As a result, a Theory-of-Mind continuum model and Theory-of-Mind Typology is proposed.
Lowther, Juliet. „Theory of mind development in deaf children“. Thesis, University of Southampton, 2003. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.398720.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleCummins, O. C. A. „Theory of mind, context processing and schizotypy“. Thesis, University College London (University of London), 2007. http://discovery.ucl.ac.uk/1445405/.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleGomes, Kathline C. „Verbal Scaffolding in Children's Theory of Mind“. Scholarship @ Claremont, 2012. http://scholarship.claremont.edu/scripps_theses/48.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleHappe, Francesca Gabrielle Elizabeth. „Theory of mind and communication in autism“. Thesis, University College London (University of London), 1991. http://discovery.ucl.ac.uk/1317758/.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleFlynn, Emma. „Theory of mind, representation and executive control“. Thesis, University of Nottingham, 2001. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.364662.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleAmin, Yazdi Seyed Amir. „Theory of mind reasoning in Iranian children“. Thesis, University of Sheffield, 2003. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.289636.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleDoherty, Martin John. „Children's theory of mind and metalinguistic awareness“. Thesis, University of Sussex, 1994. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.241689.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleFigel, Jared T. „Contemporary Functionalism and Aristotle's Theory of Mind“. Ohio University / OhioLINK, 2016. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=ohiou1460400969.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleStewart, Elizabeth Margaret. „Theory of Mind in Children with Epilepsy“. Thesis, The University of Sydney, 2019. http://hdl.handle.net/2123/20234.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleThornburg, M. Hayden. „Possibilities of mind and body an exploration and critique of mind-body identity theory /“. Theological Research Exchange Network (TREN), 2006. http://www.tren.com/search.cfm?p006-1549.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleMartin, Christopher. „Investigating the neuropsychology of theory of mind and designing a new test of theory of mind in acquired brain injury“. Thesis, University of Hull, 2008. http://hydra.hull.ac.uk/resources/hull:1363.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleMauer, Maria Christine. „Das Konstrukt der Theory of Mind bei Erwachsenen“. Diss., Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität München, 2012. http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:19-157578.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleScott, Suzanne. „Theory of mind in individuals with paranoid schizophrenia“. Thesis, University of Glasgow, 2013. http://theses.gla.ac.uk/4686/.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleDeptula, Andrew. „Autobiographical Memory and Theory of Mind in Schizotypy“. Master's thesis, University of Central Florida, 2013. http://digital.library.ucf.edu/cdm/ref/collection/ETD/id/5623.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleM.S.
Masters
Psychology
Sciences
Psychology Clinical
Corcoran, G. P. „An analysis of the computational theory of mind“. Thesis, University of Bradford, 1986. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.372172.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleGallagher, Helen Louise. „Investigating the neural correlates of 'theory of mind'“. Thesis, University College London (University of London), 2003. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.401204.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleAkande, Isaac Oluwamayowa. „Assessment of theory of mind in stroke populations“. Thesis, Canterbury Christ Church University, 2017. http://create.canterbury.ac.uk/16436/.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleSchjelderup, Vanessa. „Social anxiety : a question of theory of mind?“ Thesis, City University London, 2011. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.544444.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleFyfe, S. „Theory of mind and its relation to schizotypy“. Thesis, University College London (University of London), 2006. http://discovery.ucl.ac.uk/1444687/.
Der volle Inhalt der QuellePacer, Michael D. „Mind as Theory Engine| Causation, Explanation and Time“. Thesis, University of California, Berkeley, 2017. http://pqdtopen.proquest.com/#viewpdf?dispub=10194103.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleHumans build theories out of the data we observe, and out of those theories arise wonders. The most powerful theories are causal theories, which organise data into actionable structures. Causal theories make explicit claims about the structure of the world: what entities and processes exist in it, which of these relate to one another and in what form those relations consist. We can use causal theories to induce new generalisations about the world (in the form of particular models or other causal theories) and to explain particular occurrences. This allows rapidly disseminating causal information throughout our cognitive communities. Causal theories and the explanations derived from them guide decisions we make, including where and when to look for more data, completing the cycle.
Causal theories play a ubiquitous and potent role in everyday life, in formal pursuit of them in the sciences, and through their applications in medicine, technology and industry. Given this, the rarity of analyses that attempt to characterise causal theories and their uses in general, computational terms is surprising. Only in recent years has there been a substantial refinement of our models of causal induction due to work by computational cognitive scientists — the interdisciplinary tradition out of which which this dissertation originates. And even so, many issues related to causal theories have been left unattended; three features in particular merit much greater attention from a computational perspective: generating and evaluating explanation, the role of simplicity in explanation choice, and continuous-time causal induction. I aim to redress this situation with this dissertation.
In Chapter 0, I introduce the primary paradigms from computational cognitive science – computational level analysis and rational analysis – that govern my research. In Chapter 1, I study formal theories of causal explanation in Bayesian networks by comparing the explanations the generate and evaluate to human judgements about the same systems. No one model of causal explanation captures the pattern of human judgements, though the intuitive hypothesis, that the most probable a posteriori explanation is the best performs worst of the models evaluated. I conclude that the premise of finding model for all of human causal explanation (even in this limited domain) is flawed; the research programme should be refined to consider the features of formal models and how well they capture our explanatory practices as they vary between individuals and circumstances. One feature not expressed in these models explicitly but that has been shown to matter for human explanation is simplicity. Chapter 2 considers the problem of simplicity in human causal explanation choice in a series of four experiments. I study what makes an explanation simple (whether it is the number of causes invoked in or the number of assumptions made by an explanation), how simplicity concerns are traded off against data-fit, which cognitive consequences arise from choosing simpler explanations when the data does not fit, and why people prefer simpler explanations.
In Chapter 3, I change the focus from studying causal explanation to causal induction — in particular, I develop a framework for continuous time causal theories (
CTCTS
). ACTCT
defines a generative probabilistic framework for other generative probabilistic models of causal systems, where the data in those systems expressed in terms of continuous time. Chapter 3 is the most interdisciplinary piece of my dissertation, accordingly it begins by reviewing a number of topics: the history of theories of causal induction within philosophy, statistics and medicine; empirical work on causal induction in cognitive science, focusing on issues related to causal induction with temporal data; conceptual issues surrounding the formal definition of time, data, and causal models; and probabilistic graphical models, causal theories, and stochastic processes. I then introduce the desiderata for theCTCT
framework and how those criteria are met. I then demonstrate the power ofCTCTS
by using them to analyse five sets of experiments (some new and some derived from the literature) on human causal induction with temporal data. Bookending each experiment and the model applied to it is are case from medical history that illustrate a real-world instance of the variety of problem being solved in the section; the opening discussion describes the case and why it fits the problem structure of the model used to analyse the experimental results and the closing discussion illustrates aspects of the case omitted from the initial discussion that complicate the model and fit better with the model introduced in the next section. Then, I discuss ways to incorporate other advances in probabilistic programming, generative theories and stochastic processes into theCTCT
framework, identify potential applications with specific focus on mechanisms and feedback loops, and conclude by analysing the centrality of temporal information in the study of the mind more generally.Excepting the supporting appendices and bibliography that end the dissertation, I conclude in two parts. First, in Chapter 4, I analyse issues at the intersection of three of the main themes of my work: namely, (causal) explanation, (causal) induction and time. This proceeds by examining these topics first in pairs and then as a whole. Following that, is Chapter 5, an epilogue that clarifies the interpretations and intended meanings of the “Mind as Theory Engine” metaphor as it applies to human cognition.
Linares, Pava Leslie. „The role of culture in Theory of Mind“. Thesis, Edith Cowan University, Research Online, Perth, Western Australia, 2019. https://ro.ecu.edu.au/theses/2173.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleTrabucco-Miguel, Sandra. „La "theory of mind" dans l'accident vasculaire cérébral“. Grenoble 1, 2008. http://www.theses.fr/2008GRE10059.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleSocial intelligence and theory of mind (ToM) may reveal difficulties in social adaptation after stroke. Such difficulties may result in social and professional difficulties, leading to disturb young patients' way of life. Objective: To assess social and professional difficulties, return to work at one year, and analyze the causes. Methods: A case-control study was carried out in 74 non demented patients age-matched to 74 controls, age from 16 to 65 years, who suffered a mild to moderate stroke more than 3 months (Rankin <3). Social professional and relational difficulties were assessed using the WSAS, return to work at one year, extensive neuropsychological evaluation, and tests, questionnaries and scales to evaluate theory of mind. Results: WSAS was increased in 70,3'Yo patients and return to work was observed in 54,4'Yo. Both were predicted by depression, spatial working memoryand "faux-Pas" test to reasoning about mental states by integrating contextual and historical information about a pers on Anatomoclinical analysis showed that every theory of mind tasks were impaired in superficial middle cerebral artery strokes or in patients with lesion located in the theory of mind neuronal network, including either right superior temporal sulcus or right prefrontal cortex. The only one to be impaired, whatever stroke location, was the "faux-Pas" test, suggesting a metarepresentational process. Anxietv and depression increased theorv of mind impairment
Doostdar, Sanaje Ladan [Verfasser], und Stephanie [Akademischer Betreuer] Mehl. „Theory of Mind - Zusammenhänge von affektiven und kognitiven Theory of Mind Fähigkeiten bei Patienten mit Schizophrenie / Ladan Doostdar Sanaje ; Betreuer: Stephanie Mehl“. Marburg : Philipps-Universität Marburg, 2020. http://d-nb.info/1218685808/34.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleKallerstrup, Jesper. „On having meaning in mind“. Thesis, University of St Andrews, 2001. http://hdl.handle.net/10023/12911.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleBaimel, Adam Sean. „From keeping together in time, to keeping together in mind : behavioral synchrony and theory of mind“. Thesis, University of British Columbia, 2015. http://hdl.handle.net/2429/54339.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleArts, Faculty of
Psychology, Department of
Graduate
Fuchs, Christina. „Theory of Mind bei akut- und postpsychotischer paranoider Schizophrenie -“. Diss., lmu, 2011. http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:19-134520.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleBarnaby, B. „Schizotypy, theory of mind and the understanding of irony“. Thesis, University College London (University of London), 2006. http://discovery.ucl.ac.uk/1445309/.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleAsghar, Adam. „Theory of mind, empathy and androgyny in psychopathic disorder“. Thesis, University of Warwick, 2007. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.487651.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleDodge, Stephanie Renea. „Social dominance and theory of mind in early childhood“. Thesis, Wichita State University, 2013. http://hdl.handle.net/10057/6811.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleThesis (M.Ed.)--Wichita State University, College of Education, Dept. of Counseling, Educational Leadership, Educational and School Psychology
Glenn, Sylvia. „The development of theory of mind in deaf people“. Thesis, University of Sheffield, 2007. http://etheses.whiterose.ac.uk/3085/.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleAndriopoulos, Constantine A. „Mind stretching : a grounded theory for enhancing organisational creativity“. Thesis, University of Strathclyde, 2000. http://oleg.lib.strath.ac.uk:80/R/?func=dbin-jump-full&object_id=21175.
Der volle Inhalt der QuellePark, Sam-Yel. „A study of the mind-body theory in Spinoza“. Thesis, University of Glasgow, 1999. http://theses.gla.ac.uk/2040/.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleWilliams, Claire. „Persecutory delusions, schizotypy and disruptions to theory of mind“. Thesis, University College London (University of London), 2006. http://discovery.ucl.ac.uk/1446421/.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleMisailidi, Plousia. „Social behaviour and theory of mind deficits in autism“. Thesis, University College London (University of London), 1996. http://discovery.ucl.ac.uk/10020269/.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleFuller, Timothy. „Science and Mind: How theory change illuminates ordinary thought“. The Ohio State University, 2012. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=osu1343840173.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleCurry, Ryan H. „CHILDREN’S THEORY OF MIND, JOINT ATTENTION, AND VIDEO CHAT“. Case Western Reserve University School of Graduate Studies / OhioLINK, 2021. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=case1616663322967054.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleHirao, Kazuyuki. „Theory of mind and frontal lobe pathology in schizophrenia“. Kyoto University, 2008. http://hdl.handle.net/2433/124245.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleHastie, Natalie. „A computational theory of world: Mind in Leibnizian metaphysics“. Thesis, Hastie, Natalie (2014) A computational theory of world: Mind in Leibnizian metaphysics. Honours thesis, Murdoch University, 2014. https://researchrepository.murdoch.edu.au/id/eprint/25125/.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleYoon, Jeung Eun. „Theory of mind in middle childhood : assessment and prediction“. Diss., University of Iowa, 2015. https://ir.uiowa.edu/etd/1941.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleGrape, Amelie, und Sara Sandstig. „Theory of mind, språkliga förmågor och ickeverbal intelligens hos barn mellan tre och fyra års ålder : Översättning och validering av Theory of Mind Scale“. Thesis, Linköpings universitet, Logopedi, 2012. http://urn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:liu:diva-79309.
Der volle Inhalt der QuelleTheory of mind is an ability to ascribe other people mental states to explain their behaviors. These mental states may regard beliefs, intentions or feelings. Theory of Mind Scale is an evaluation scale designed by Wellman and Liu (2004). The scale consists of different scale steps of theory of mind, with an increasing difficulty. The purpose of this study was to translate Theory of Mind Scale from English to Swedish and validate the scale for children from the age of 3 to 4 years. The study also aims to examine if there is any relationship between children’s theory of mind, grammatical understanding, receptive vocabulary and non-verbal intelligence. The validation of the present study showed that the order of the scale steps in the American original version of the Theory of Mind Scale was applicable to the children in the present study, with an exeption of the last task (real-apparent emotion). An alternative to the scale would be to compare the children’s performance against a normal variation. According to the present study a child between the age of 3;0 and 3;11 years could be expected to complete 2,83 ± 2,50 tasks and a child between the age of 4;0 and 4;11 years could be expected to complete 3,71 ± 1,50 tasks. The results indicated a stronger correlation between theory of mind and non-verbal inteligence than between theory of mind and receptive vocabulary in children between the age of 3;0 and 4;11 years. No significant correlation existed between receptive grammar and theory of mind. The translation and validation of Theory of Mind Scale enables qualitative clinical investigations of theory of mind deficits in children.