Um die anderen Arten von Veröffentlichungen zu diesem Thema anzuzeigen, folgen Sie diesem Link: Spatial neglekt.

Zeitschriftenartikel zum Thema „Spatial neglekt“

Geben Sie eine Quelle nach APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard und anderen Zitierweisen an

Wählen Sie eine Art der Quelle aus:

Machen Sie sich mit Top-50 Zeitschriftenartikel für die Forschung zum Thema "Spatial neglekt" bekannt.

Neben jedem Werk im Literaturverzeichnis ist die Option "Zur Bibliographie hinzufügen" verfügbar. Nutzen Sie sie, wird Ihre bibliographische Angabe des gewählten Werkes nach der nötigen Zitierweise (APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver usw.) automatisch gestaltet.

Sie können auch den vollen Text der wissenschaftlichen Publikation im PDF-Format herunterladen und eine Online-Annotation der Arbeit lesen, wenn die relevanten Parameter in den Metadaten verfügbar sind.

Sehen Sie die Zeitschriftenartikel für verschiedene Spezialgebieten durch und erstellen Sie Ihre Bibliographie auf korrekte Weise.

1

Nasonova, Tetiana, Tetiana Kolosova, Danylo Briakin, Natalia Mykhailovska, Igor Kurochkin, Tetiana Riabychenko, Olena Tyshkevych, Yurii Buhaiov, Yurii Holovchenko und Valeriia Hushcha. „Neglect syndrome after ischemic stroke: diagnosis, treatment options“. Ukrains'kyi Visnyk Psykhonevrolohii 27, Nr. 2 (99) (10.06.2019): 8–14. http://dx.doi.org/10.36927/2079-0325-v27-is2-2019-2.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
Annotation:
The purpose of the study: the study of clinical efficacy and safety of the Neuromidin® in the rehabilitation period in patients with primary ischemic stroke, its eff ect on motor, cognitive function, level of household adaptation, depending on the presence or absence of neglekt syndrome and "push-syndrome". 60 patients aged 49 to 82 years in the recovery period of ischemic stroke were examined (30 patients — the main group: treatment with the addition of Neuromidin®; 30 patients — control group: standard treatment without adding Neuromidin®). Investigation of motor, cognitive, and sensitive disorders was carried out with the use of clinical scales. The effectiveness of the treatment was evaluated in the absence of signs of neglekt syndrome and "push-syndrome", reduction of motor and cognitive impairment. Patients with neglekt syndrome showed greater dependence on third-party care and more severe cognitive impairment than in patients who had not been diagnosed neglekt syndrome. After treatment, the majori ty of patients achieved elimination of neglekt syndrome, but in the group where patients received Neuromidin®, the number of such patients was higher. In addition, patients in the main group improved cognitive function. The use of Neuromidin® in complex rehabilitation measures in patients with ischemic stroke contributes to the restoration of lost functions, reduces the phenomena of visual-spatial neglect, positively affects self-care and improves the quality of life of patients and their relatives.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
2

Silver, Bernie. „Spatial Neglect“. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation 16, Nr. 6 (Dezember 2001): 613–14. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00001199-200112000-00011.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
3

Li, Korina, und Paresh A. Malhotra. „Spatial neglect“. Practical Neurology 15, Nr. 5 (28.05.2015): 333–39. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/practneurol-2015-001115.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
4

Kirk, Andrew. „Spatial neglect“. Current Neurology and Neuroscience Reports 1, Nr. 6 (November 2001): 541–46. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11910-001-0059-x.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
5

Maeda, Masaharu. „Unilateral spatial neglect“. Higher Brain Function Research 28, Nr. 2 (2008): 214–23. http://dx.doi.org/10.2496/hbfr.28.214.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
6

Mark, V. W., und K. M. Heilman. „Diagonal spatial neglect“. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry 65, Nr. 3 (01.09.1998): 348–52. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.65.3.348.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
7

Ishiai, Sumio. „Unilateral spatial neglect“. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation 4, Nr. 2 (Juni 1994): 143–46. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09602019408402272.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
8

Swan, Laurie. „Unilateral Spatial Neglect“. Physical Therapy 81, Nr. 9 (01.09.2001): 1572–80. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/ptj/81.9.1572.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
9

Shimizu, Taro, Tomoya Abe, Yasutoshi Akasaki und Hideaki Kamiishi. „Unilateral spatial neglect“. BMJ Case Reports 13, Nr. 11 (November 2020): e239770. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/bcr-2020-239770.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
10

Cubelli, Roberto. „Definition: Spatial neglect“. Cortex 92 (Juli 2017): 320–21. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2017.03.021.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
11

Karnath, Hans-Otto. „Spatial attention systems in spatial neglect“. Neuropsychologia 75 (August 2015): 61–73. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.05.019.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
12

Malhotra, P. A., D. Soto, K. Li und C. Russell. „Reward modulates spatial neglect“. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry 84, Nr. 4 (15.10.2012): 366–69. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/jnnp-2012-303169.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
13

Posner, Michael I. „Neglect and spatial attention“. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation 4, Nr. 2 (Juni 1994): 183–87. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09602019408402280.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
14

Cubelli, Roberto, Sergio Della Sala, Nicoletta Beschin und Robert D. McIntosh. „Distance-mediated spatial neglect“. Neurocase 20, Nr. 3 (03.04.2013): 338–45. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13554794.2013.770885.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
15

L, A. „Dissociation of visuo-spatial neglect and neglect dyslexia“. Neurocase 1, Nr. 3 (01.09.1995): 209a—216. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/neucas/1.3.209-a.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
16

Maeshima, Shinichiro. „Subclassification of unilateral spatial neglect“. Higher Brain Function Research 26, Nr. 3 (2006): 235–44. http://dx.doi.org/10.2496/hbfr.26.235.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
17

Speedie, Lynn J., Eli Wertman, Mieke Verfaellie, Charles Butter, Nechama Silberman, Micky Liechtenstein und Kenneth M. Heilman. „Reading Direction and Spatial Neglect“. Cortex 38, Nr. 1 (Januar 2002): 59–67. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0010-9452(08)70638-5.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
18

MIJOVIĆ, DANICA. „MECHANISMS OF VISUAL SPATIAL NEGLECT“. Brain 114, Nr. 4 (1991): 1575–93. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/114.4.1575.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
19

Roux, Franck-Emmanuel, Olivier Dufor, Valérie Lauwers-Cances, Leila Boukhatem, David Brauge, Louisa Draper, Jean-Albert Lotterie und Jean-François Démonet. „Electrostimulation Mapping of Spatial Neglect“. Neurosurgery 69, Nr. 6 (28.06.2011): 1218–31. http://dx.doi.org/10.1227/neu.0b013e31822aefd2.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
Annotation:
Abstract BACKGROUND Cortical and subcortical electrostimulation mapping during awake brain surgery for tumor removal is usually used to minimize deficits. OBJECTIVE To use electrostimulation to study neuronal substrates involved in spatial awareness in humans. METHODS Spatial neglect was studied using a line bisection task in combination with electrostimulation mapping of the right hemisphere in 50 cases. Stimulation sites were identified with Talairach coordinates. The behavioral effects induced by stimulation, especially eye movements and deviations from the median, were quantified and compared with preoperative data and a control group. RESULTS Composite and highly individualized spatial neglect maps were generated. Both rightward and leftward deviations were induced, sometimes in the same patient but for different stimulation sites. Group analysis showed that specific and reproducible line deviations were induced by stimulation of discrete cortical areas located in the posterior part of the right superior and middle temporal gyri, inferior parietal lobe, and inferior postcentral and inferior frontal gyri (P > .05). Fiber tracking identified stimulated subcortical areas important to spare as sections of fronto-occipital and superior longitudinal II fascicles. According to preoperative and postoperative neglect battery tests, the specificity and sensitivity of intraoperative line bisection tests were 94% and 83%, respectively. CONCLUSION In humans, discrete cortical areas that are variable in location between individuals but mainly located within the right posterior Sylvian fissure sustain visuospatial attention specifically toward the contralateral or ipsilateral space direction. Line bisection mapping was found to be a reliable method for minimizing spatial neglect caused by brain tumor surgery.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
20

Bowen, Audrey, Nadina B. Lincoln und Michael E. Dewey. „Spatial Neglect: Is Rehabilitation Effective?“ Stroke 33, Nr. 11 (November 2002): 2728–29. http://dx.doi.org/10.1161/01.str.0000035747.03607.1a.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
21

Karnath, Hans-Otto, und Christopher Rorden. „The anatomy of spatial neglect“. Neuropsychologia 50, Nr. 6 (Mai 2012): 1010–17. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.06.027.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
22

Walker, Robin. „Spatial and object-based neglect“. Neurocase 1, Nr. 4 (Oktober 1995): 371–83. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13554799508402381.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
23

Karnath, Hans-Otto, und Marianne Dieterich. „Spatial neglect—a vestibular disorder?“ Brain 129, Nr. 2 (21.12.2005): 293–305. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/awh698.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
24

Vuilleumier, Patrik, und Theodor Landis. „Illusory contours and spatial neglect“. NeuroReport 9, Nr. 11 (August 1998): 2481–84. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00001756-199808030-00010.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
25

Keller, Ingo, Jochen Ditterich, Thomas Eggert und Andreas Straube. „Size distortion in spatial neglect“. NeuroReport 11, Nr. 8 (Juni 2000): 1655–60. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00001756-200006050-00012.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
26

Corbetta, Maurizio, und Gordon L. Shulman. „Spatial Neglect and Attention Networks“. Annual Review of Neuroscience 34, Nr. 1 (21.07.2011): 569–99. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev-neuro-061010-113731.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
27

Hillis, Argye E. „Neurobiology of Unilateral Spatial Neglect“. Neuroscientist 12, Nr. 2 (April 2006): 153–63. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1073858405284257.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
28

Terrell, Beth-Marie, Christine Towler und A. M. Barrett. „Aphasia-accessible Spatial Neglect Care“. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation 101, Nr. 11 (November 2020): e32. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.apmr.2020.09.090.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
29

Mizuno, Katsuhiro. „Rehabilitation for Unilateral Spatial Neglect“. Japanese Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine 58, Nr. 1 (18.01.2021): 53–58. http://dx.doi.org/10.2490/jjrmc.58.53.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
30

Rusconi, Maria Luisa, und Laura Carelli. „Long-Term Efficacy of Prism Adaptation on Spatial Neglect: Preliminary Results on Different Spatial Components“. Scientific World Journal 2012 (2012): 1–8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1100/2012/618528.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
Annotation:
This study describes the long-term effectiveness on spatial neglect recovery of a 2-week treatment based on prism adaptation (PA). Seven right-brain-damaged patients affected by chronic neglect were evaluated before, after two weeks of the PA treatment and at a follow-up (variable between 8 and 30 months after the end of PA). Neglect evaluation was performed by means of BIT (conventional and behavioral), Fluff Test, and Comb and Razor Test. The results highlight an improvement, after the PA training, in both tasks performed using the hand trained in PA treatment and in behavioral tasks not requiring a manual motor response. Such effects extend, even if not significantly, to all BIT subtests. These results support previous findings, showing that PA improves neglect also on imagery tasks with no manual component, and provide further evidence for long-lasting efficacy of PA training. Dissociations have been found with regard to PA efficacy on peripersonal, personal, and representational neglect, visuospatial agraphia and neglect dyslexia. In particular, we found no significant differences between the pre-training and post-training PA session in personal neglect measures, and a poor recovery of neglect dyslexia after PA treatment. The recruitment of a larger sample could help to confirm the effectiveness of the prismatic lenses with regard to the different clinical manifestations of spatial neglect.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
31

Bonato, Mario, Arnaud Saj und Patrik Vuilleumier. „Hemispatial Neglect Shows That “Before” Is “Left”“. Neural Plasticity 2016 (2016): 1–11. http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2016/2716036.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
Annotation:
Recent research has led to the hypothesis that events which unfold in time might be spatially represented in a left-to-right fashion, resembling writing direction. Here we studied fourteen right-hemisphere damaged patients, with or without neglect, a disorder of spatial awareness affecting contralesional (here left) space processing and representation. We reasoned that if the processing of time-ordered events is spatial in nature, it should be impaired in the presence of neglect and spared in its absence. Patients categorized events of a story as occurring before or after a central event, which acted as a temporal reference. An asymmetric distance effect emerged in neglect patients, with slower responses to events that took place before the temporal reference. The event occurring immediately before the reference elicited particularly slow responses, closely mirroring the pattern found in neglect patients performing numerical comparison tasks. Moreover, the first item elicited significantly slower responses than the last one, suggesting a preference for a left-to-right scanning/representation of events in time. Patients without neglect showed a regular and symmetric distance effect. These findings further suggest that the representation of events order is spatial in nature and provide compelling evidence that ordinality is similarly represented within temporal and numerical domains.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
32

Becchio, Cristina, und Cesare Bertone. „Time and neglect: Abnormal temporal dynamics in unilateral spatial neglect“. Neuropsychologia 44, Nr. 14 (Januar 2006): 2775–82. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.06.011.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
33

Hillis, Argye E., Brenda Rapp, Lisa Benzing und Alfonso Caramazza. „Dissociable Coordinate Frames of Unilateral Spatial Neglect: “Viewer-Centered” Neglect“. Brain and Cognition 37, Nr. 3 (August 1998): 491–526. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/brcg.1998.1010.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
34

Chen, Peii, Irene Ward, Ummais Khan, Yan Liu und Kimberly Hreha. „Spatial Neglect Hinders Success of Inpatient Rehabilitation in Individuals With Traumatic Brain Injury“. Neurorehabilitation and Neural Repair 30, Nr. 5 (03.09.2015): 451–60. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1545968315604397.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
Annotation:
Background. Current knowledge about spatial neglect and its impact on rehabilitation mostly originates from stroke studies. Objective. To examine the impact of spatial neglect on rehabilitation outcome in individuals with traumatic brain injury (TBI). Methods. The retrospective study included 156 consecutive patients with TBI (73 women; median age = 69.5 years; interquartile range = 50-81 years) at an inpatient rehabilitation facility (IRF). We examined whether the presence of spatial neglect affected the Functional Independence Measure (FIM) scores, length of stay, or discharge disposition. Based on the available medical records, we also explored whether spatial neglect was associated with tactile sensation or muscle strength asymmetry in the extremities and whether specific brain injuries or lesions predicted spatial neglect. Results. In all, 30.1% (47 of 156) of the sample had spatial neglect. Sex, age, severity of TBI, or time postinjury did not differ between patients with and without spatial neglect. In comparison to patients without spatial neglect, patients with the disorder stayed in IRF 5 days longer, had lower FIM scores at discharge, improved slower in both Cognitive and Motor FIM scores, and might have less likelihood of return home. In addition, left-sided neglect was associated with asymmetric strength in the lower extremities, specifically left weaker than the right. Finally, brain injury–induced mass effect predicted left-sided neglect. Conclusions. Spatial neglect is common following TBI, impedes rehabilitation progress in both motor and cognitive domains, and prolongs length of stay. Future research is needed for linking specific traumatic injuries and lesioned networks to spatial neglect and related impairment.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
35

Saj, Arnaud, Juliane C. Wilcke, Markus Gschwind, Héloïse Emond und Frédéric Assal. „Spatial Hyperschematia without Spatial Neglect after Insulo-Thalamic Disconnection“. PLoS ONE 8, Nr. 11 (26.11.2013): e79938. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0079938.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
36

Kerkhoff, Georg, Frank Artinger und Wolfram Ziegler. „Contrasting spatial hearing deficits in hemianopia and spatial neglect“. NeuroReport 10, Nr. 17 (November 1999): 3555–60. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00001756-199911260-00017.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
37

Chen, Xiao-Wei, Cheng-He Lin, Hua Zheng und Zhen-Lan Lin. „A Chinese Patient with Pusher Syndrome and Unilateral Spatial Neglect Syndrome“. Canadian Journal of Neurological Sciences / Journal Canadien des Sciences Neurologiques 41, Nr. 4 (Juli 2014): 493–97. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0317167100018540.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
Annotation:
Objective:To observe clinical manifestations, behavioral characteristics, and effects of rehabilitation on a patient with pusher syndrome and unilateral spatial neglect caused by right thalamic hemorrhage.Methods:Assessment of pusher syndrome was made by the Scale for Contraversive pushing (SCP), and unilateral spatial neglect syndrome was diagnosed using line cancellation, letter and star cancellation, line bisection tests and copy and continuation of graphic sequence test. Behavioral therapy, occupational therapy, reading training and traditional Chinese medicine methods were adopted for treatment of pusher syndrome and unilateral spatial neglect.Results:The patient showed typical pusher syndrome and unilateral spatial neglect symptoms. The pusher syndrome and unilateral spatial neglect symptoms were significantly improved following rehabilitation treatments.Conclusions:Pusher syndrome and unilateral spatial neglect syndrome occurred simultaneously after right thalamic hemorrhage. Early rehabilitation therapy can reduce the symptoms of pusher syndrome and unilateral spatial neglect syndrome and improve motor function.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
38

Ishiai, Sumio. „Rehabilitation approaches to unilateral spatial neglect“. Higher Brain Function Research 28, Nr. 3 (2008): 247–56. http://dx.doi.org/10.2496/hbfr.28.247.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
39

Ishiai, Sumio. „Visual world of unilateral spatial neglect“. Higher Brain Function Research 30, Nr. 1 (2010): 1–9. http://dx.doi.org/10.2496/hbfr.30.1.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
40

Halligan, Peter W., Gereon R. Fink, John C. Marshall und Giuseppe Vallar. „Spatial cognition: evidence from visual neglect“. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7, Nr. 3 (März 2003): 125–33. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s1364-6613(03)00032-9.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
41

Suzuki, Eiji, Wuxiong Chen und Tohru Kondo. „Measuring unilateral spatial neglect during stepping“. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation 78, Nr. 2 (Februar 1997): 173–78. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0003-9993(97)90260-4.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
42

GILAD, R., M. SADEH, M. BOAZ und Y. LAMPL. „Visual Spatial Neglect in Multiple Sclerosis“. Cortex 42, Nr. 8 (2006): 1138–42. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0010-9452(08)70226-0.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
43

Chokron, Sylvie, Eve Dupierrix, Matthias Tabert und Paolo Bartolomeo. „Experimental remission of unilateral spatial neglect“. Neuropsychologia 45, Nr. 14 (Januar 2007): 3127–48. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.08.001.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
44

Yoo, Woo-Kyoung. „Subtypes of Spatial Neglect and Assessment“. Brain & Neurorehabilitation 2, Nr. 1 (2009): 46. http://dx.doi.org/10.12786/bn.2009.2.1.46.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
45

Laurent-Vannier, A., P. Pradat-Diehl, M. Chevignard, G. Abada und M. De Agostini. „Spatial and motor neglect in children“. Neurology 60, Nr. 2 (28.01.2003): 202–7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1212/01.wnl.0000048201.26494.0b.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
46

Broadway, Amy. „Eye Patching in Unilateral Spatial Neglect“. Neurology Report 23, Nr. 3 (1999): 129–30. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/01253086-199923030-00018.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
47

Gainotti, Guido. „The dilemma of unilateral spatial neglect“. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation 4, Nr. 2 (Juni 1994): 127–32. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09602019408402269.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
48

Milner, A. David, und Monika Harvey. „Toward a taxonomy of spatial neglect“. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation 4, Nr. 2 (Juni 1994): 177–81. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09602019408402279.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
49

Beis, J. M., J. M. André, A. Barre und J. Paysant. „Mirror images and unilateral spatial neglect“. Neuropsychologia 39, Nr. 13 (Januar 2001): 1444–50. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0028-3932(01)00065-3.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
50

Adair, John C., und Anna M. Barrett. „Spatial Neglect: Clinical and Neuroscience Review“. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1142, Nr. 1 (Oktober 2008): 21–43. http://dx.doi.org/10.1196/annals.1444.008.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
Wir bieten Rabatte auf alle Premium-Pläne für Autoren, deren Werke in thematische Literatursammlungen aufgenommen wurden. Kontaktieren Sie uns, um einen einzigartigen Promo-Code zu erhalten!

Zur Bibliographie