Um die anderen Arten von Veröffentlichungen zu diesem Thema anzuzeigen, folgen Sie diesem Link: Human information processing.

Zeitschriftenartikel zum Thema „Human information processing“

Geben Sie eine Quelle nach APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard und anderen Zitierweisen an

Wählen Sie eine Art der Quelle aus:

Machen Sie sich mit Top-50 Zeitschriftenartikel für die Forschung zum Thema "Human information processing" bekannt.

Neben jedem Werk im Literaturverzeichnis ist die Option "Zur Bibliographie hinzufügen" verfügbar. Nutzen Sie sie, wird Ihre bibliographische Angabe des gewählten Werkes nach der nötigen Zitierweise (APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver usw.) automatisch gestaltet.

Sie können auch den vollen Text der wissenschaftlichen Publikation im PDF-Format herunterladen und eine Online-Annotation der Arbeit lesen, wenn die relevanten Parameter in den Metadaten verfügbar sind.

Sehen Sie die Zeitschriftenartikel für verschiedene Spezialgebieten durch und erstellen Sie Ihre Bibliographie auf korrekte Weise.

1

CALLISTA ROY, SISTER. „Human Information Processing“. Annual Review of Nursing Research 6, Nr. 1 (September 1988): 237–62. http://dx.doi.org/10.1891/0739-6686.6.1.237.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
2

Callaway, Enoch, Roy Halliday, Hilary Naylor, Lovelle Yano und Karen Herzig. „Drugs and Human Information Processing“. Neuropsychopharmacology 10, Nr. 1 (Februar 1994): 9–19. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/npp.1994.2.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
3

Velmans, Max. „Is human information processing conscious?“ Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14, Nr. 4 (Dezember 1991): 651–69. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x00071776.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
Annotation:
AbstractInvestigations of the function of consciousness in human information processing have focused mainly on two questions: (1) Where does consciousness enter into the information processing sequence, and (2) how does conscious processing differ from preconscious and unconscious processing? Input analysis is thought to be initially “preconscious” and “pre-attentive” - fast, involuntary, and automatic. This is followed by “conscious,” “focal-attentive” analysis, which is relatively slow, voluntary, and flexible. It is thought that simple, familiar stimuli can be identified preconsciously, but conscious processing is needed to identify complex, novel stimuli. Conscious processing has also been thought to be necessary for choice, learning and memory, and the organization of complex, novel responses, particularly those requiring planning, reflection, or creativity.The present target article reviews evidence that consciousness performs none of these functions. Consciousness nearly alwaysresultsfrom focal-attentive processing (as a form of output) but does not itselfenter intothis or any other form of human information processing. This suggests that the term “conscious process” needs reexamination. Consciousnessappearsto be necessary in a variety of tasks because they require focal-attentive processing; if consciousness is absent, focal-attentive processing is absent. From afirst-person perspective, however, conscious statesarecausally effective. First-person accounts arecomplementaryto third-person accounts. Although they can be translated into third-person accounts, they cannot be reduced to them.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
4

Petrie, Rachel X. A., und Ian J. Deary. „Smoking and human information processing“. Psychopharmacology 99, Nr. 3 (November 1989): 393–96. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/bf00445565.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
5

Spangenberg, J. F. A., und F. J. N. Nijhuis. „Human information processing in science“. Scientometrics 18, Nr. 5-6 (Mai 1990): 389–407. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/bf02020153.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
6

Rogers, Tracy. „Human information processing under stress“. Journal of Emergency Management 21, Nr. 2 (16.05.2023): 141–54. http://dx.doi.org/10.5055/jem.0756.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
Annotation:
This article is a review of literature focused on how human beings process information under stress. Three major theories on information processing are reviewed: cue utilization theory, attentional control theory, and working memory capacity theory. Different conditions that can cause an individual to feel stress are examined, how stress affects information processing, ways which stress may be beneficial, and different ways to mitigate stress, so they may process information more accurately and efficiently. Throughout the article, examples of how stress can affect incident commanders responding to a disaster situation are used to illustrate research findings.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
7

Ishii, Hiroshi. „The Dawn of Kansei Information Processing. Human Interface and 'Kansei' Information Processing.“ Journal of the Institute of Image Information and Television Engineers 52, Nr. 1 (1998): 56–59. http://dx.doi.org/10.3169/itej.52.56.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
8

Lynn, Christopher W., Lia Papadopoulos, Ari E. Kahn und Danielle S. Bassett. „Human information processing in complex networks“. Nature Physics 16, Nr. 9 (15.06.2020): 965–73. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41567-020-0924-7.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
9

PAAS, FRED G. W. C., und JOS J. ADAM. „Human information processing during physical exercise“. Ergonomics 34, Nr. 11 (November 1991): 1385–97. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00140139108964879.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
10

Robertson, Ivan T. „Human information-processing strategies and style“. Behaviour & Information Technology 4, Nr. 1 (Januar 1985): 19–29. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/01449298508901784.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
11

Fedzechkina, Maryia, Becky Chu und T. Florian Jaeger. „Human Information Processing Shapes Language Change“. Psychological Science 29, Nr. 1 (01.12.2017): 72–82. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797617728726.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
Annotation:
Human languages exhibit both striking diversity and abstract commonalities. Whether these commonalities are shaped by potentially universal principles of human information processing has been of central interest in the language and psychological sciences. Research has identified one such abstract property in the domain of word order: Although sentence word-order preferences vary across languages, the superficially different orders result in short grammatical dependencies between words. Because dependencies are easier to process when they are short rather than long, these findings raise the possibility that languages are shaped by biases of human information processing. In the current study, we directly tested the hypothesized causal link. We found that learners exposed to novel miniature artificial languages that had unnecessarily long dependencies did not follow the surface preference of their native language but rather systematically restructured the input to reduce dependency lengths. These results provide direct evidence for a causal link between processing preferences in individual speakers and patterns in linguistic diversity.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
12

Callaway, Enoch. „The Biology of Human Information Processing“. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs 18, Nr. 4 (Oktober 1986): 315–18. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02791072.1986.10472363.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
13

Rugg, Michael D. „Psychophysiological approaches to human information processing“. Biological Psychology 22, Nr. 2 (April 1986): 208–9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0301-0511(86)90073-6.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
14

Wilkinson, Thomas M. „Human Information Processing, Health Information Technology, and Medical Outcomes“. JAMA 302, Nr. 13 (07.10.2009): 1417. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/jama.2009.1417.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
15

Ramsey, N. F. „Neurophysiological factors in human information processing capacity“. Brain 127, Nr. 3 (07.11.2003): 517–25. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/awh060.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
16

Jayles, Bertrand, Ramón Escobedo, Roberto Pasqua, Christophe Zanon, Adrien Blanchet, Matthieu Roy, Gilles Tredan, Guy Theraulaz und Clément Sire. „Collective information processing in human phase separation“. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 375, Nr. 1807 (27.07.2020): 20190801. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2019.0801.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
Annotation:
In our digital societies, individuals massively interact through digital interfaces whose impact on collective dynamics can be important. In particular, the combination of social media filters and recommender systems can lead to the emergence of polarized and fragmented groups. In some social contexts, such segregation processes of human groups have been shown to share similarities with phase separation phenomena in physics. Here, we study the impact of information filtering on collective segregation behaviour of human groups. We report a series of experiments where groups of 22 subjects have to perform a collective segregation task that mimics the tendency of individuals to bond with other similar individuals. More precisely, the participants are each assigned a colour (red or blue) unknown to them, and have to regroup with other subjects sharing the same colour. To assist them, they are equipped with an artificial sensory device capable of detecting the majority colour in their ‘environment’ (defined as their k nearest neighbours, unbeknownst to them), for which we control the perception range, k = 1, 3, 5, 7, 9, 11, 13. We study the separation dynamics (emergence of unicolour groups) and the properties of the final state, and show that the value of k controls the quality of the segregation, although the subjects are totally unaware of the precise definition of the ‘environment’. We also find that there is a perception range k = 7 above which the ability of the group to segregate does not improve. We introduce a model that precisely describes the random motion of a group of pedestrians in a confined space, and which faithfully reproduces and allows interpretation of the results of the segregation experiments. Finally, we discuss the strong and precise analogy between our experiment and the phase separation of two immiscible materials at very low temperature. This article is part of the theme issue ‘Multi-scale analysis and modelling of collective migration in biological systems’.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
17

Spivey, Michael J. „Redesigning our theories of human information processing“. Discourse, Cognition and Communication 15, Nr. 3 (12.12.2007): 261–65. http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/idj.15.3.08spi.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
18

KITAGAWA, NORIMICHI, und CHARLES SPENCE. „Audiotactile multisensory interactions in human information processing“. Japanese Psychological Research 48, Nr. 3 (September 2006): 158–73. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-5884.2006.00317.x.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
19

Dickhaut, John, Vernon Smith, Baohua Xin und Aldo Rustichini. „Human economic choice as costly information processing“. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization 94 (Oktober 2013): 206–21. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2013.08.001.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
20

Sanders, Andries F. „Human information processing: Measures, mechanisms and models“. Acta Psychologica 76, Nr. 2 (April 1991): 199–200. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0001-6918(91)90048-5.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
21

Ren, Fuji. „Affective Information Processing and Recognizing Human Emotion“. Electronic Notes in Theoretical Computer Science 225 (Januar 2009): 39–50. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.entcs.2008.12.065.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
22

Duncan, James R. „Human Information Processing, Health Information Technology, and Medical Outcomes—Reply“. JAMA 302, Nr. 13 (07.10.2009): 1417. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/jama.2009.1418.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
23

Alvandi, Ebrahim Oshni. „Emotions and Information Processing“. International Journal of Synthetic Emotions 2, Nr. 1 (Januar 2011): 1–14. http://dx.doi.org/10.4018/jse.2011010101.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
Annotation:
An animate system standing in nature and trying to investigate its surroundings for different purposes does a type of cognitive processing. Emotions as mental states are leading human cognitive features that attract life by interactions processed in the world. This paper examines how this cognitive feature process works. By researching history and theories related to emotions and their generation, it becomes clear that information processing is discussed as a tool for their processes. Three different styles of information processing are evaluated for emotional processes. The pragmatic notion of information processing fits as a processing tool in modeling emotions and artificial emotions and explains the emotional process.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
24

Rikani, Azadeh A., Zia Choudhry, Adnan Maqsood Choudhry, Nasir Rizvi und Huma Ikram. „Spatial information processing by the human visual system“. El Mednifico Journal 2, Nr. 3 (25.07.2014): 309. http://dx.doi.org/10.18035/emj.v2i3.156.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
25

Lynn, Christopher W., Lia Papadopoulos, Ari E. Kahn und Danielle S. Bassett. „Author Correction: Human information processing in complex networks“. Nature Physics 16, Nr. 12 (07.07.2020): 1238. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41567-020-0985-7.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
26

Driver, Michael J., Katarina Svensson, Roy P. Amato und Larry E. Pate. „A Human-Information-Processing Approach to Strategic Change“. International Studies of Management & Organization 26, Nr. 1 (März 1996): 41–58. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00208825.1996.11656673.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
27

No authorship indicated. „Review of Eye Movements and Human Information Processing.“ Contemporary Psychology: A Journal of Reviews 31, Nr. 10 (Oktober 1986): 820. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/024205.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
28

Lounasmaa, O. V., M. Hamalainen, R. Hari und R. Salmelin. „Information processing in the human brain: magnetoencephalographic approach.“ Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 93, Nr. 17 (20.08.1996): 8809–15. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.93.17.8809.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
29

Truta, Meda, und Mihai Popescu. „Intelligent Control System Based on Human Information Processing“. IFAC Proceedings Volumes 34, Nr. 3 (Mai 2001): 117–20. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s1474-6670(17)34336-7.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
30

Howes, Andrew, Richard L. Lewis und Satinder Singh. „Utility Maximization and Bounds on Human Information Processing“. Topics in Cognitive Science 6, Nr. 2 (20.03.2014): 198–203. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/tops.12089.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
31

Seo, Chang-Jin. „Artificial Vision System using Human Visual Information Processing“. Journal of Digital Convergence 12, Nr. 11 (28.11.2014): 349–55. http://dx.doi.org/10.14400/jdc.2014.12.11.349.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
32

Coles, MichaelG H. „Psychophysiology and contemporary models of human information processing“. Biological Psychology 20, Nr. 3 (Mai 1985): 189–90. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0301-0511(85)90066-3.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
33

Ramanujan, S., und RB Cooper. „A human information processing perspective on software maintenance“. Omega 22, Nr. 2 (März 1994): 185–203. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0305-0483(94)90078-7.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
34

Czigler, István, Gergely Csibra und Ágnes Ambró. „Age and Information Processing“. European Psychologist 2, Nr. 3 (Januar 1997): 247–57. http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1016-9040.2.3.247.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
Annotation:
This paper reviews our recent studies on the effects of aging on human information processing. In these studies the event-related potentials of the brain (ERPs) recorded in visual discrimination tasks were compared in younger and older groups of subjects in four experiments. We obtained a slight age-related delay of the NA component of the ERP. This component is a correlate of elementary pattern-identification processes. Obvious latency differences appeared on the anterior positivity, selection negativity, and N2b components in tasks where the target stimuli were defined by two stimulus characteristics. These components are correlates of attentional processes, i.e., the results support the view emphasizing age-related decline of the attentional processes. In the elderly the late positivity was less sensitive to stimulus probability, and in the older groups this component was more evenly distributed over the scalp. These results are considered as an indication that the structure of stimulus sequences was less efficiently represented in the older subjects.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
35

Yang, C. Y. David, und Jon D. Fricker. „Using Human Information Processing Principles to Design Advanced Traveler Information Systems“. Transportation Research Record: Journal of the Transportation Research Board 1759, Nr. 1 (Januar 2001): 1–8. http://dx.doi.org/10.3141/1759-01.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
36

Fagan, Joseph, und Robert E. Ployhart. „The information processing foundations of human capital resources: Leveraging insights from information processing approaches to intelligence“. Human Resource Management Review 25, Nr. 1 (März 2015): 4–11. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.hrmr.2014.09.003.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
37

Diamant, Emanuel. „Modeling human-like intelligent image processing: An information processing perspective and approach“. Signal Processing: Image Communication 22, Nr. 6 (Juli 2007): 583–90. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.image.2007.05.007.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
38

Chakarov, Vihren E., Krassimir T. Atanassov und Anthony G. Shannon. „Generalized Net Model of Auditory Information Processing“. Journal of Advanced Computational Intelligence and Intelligent Informatics 11, Nr. 5 (20.06.2007): 452–56. http://dx.doi.org/10.20965/jaciii.2007.p0452.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
Annotation:
In a series of papers we have described the human body, its separate systems, and their behavior using Generalized Nets (GNs). Here we construct a GN-model that represents auditory information processing - the first on our research on the structure and behavior of the human nervous system.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
39

Jackson, Russell E., und Dustin P. Calvillo. „Evolutionary Relevance Facilitates Visual Information Processing“. Evolutionary Psychology 11, Nr. 5 (Dezember 2013): 147470491301100. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/147470491301100506.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
Annotation:
Visual search of the environment is a fundamental human behavior that perceptual load affects powerfully. Previously investigated means for overcoming the inhibitions of high perceptual load, however, generalize poorly to real-world human behavior. We hypothesized that humans would process evolutionarily relevant stimuli more efficiently than evolutionarily novel stimuli, and evolutionary relevance would mitigate the repercussions of high perceptual load during visual search. Animacy is a significant component to evolutionary relevance of visual stimuli because perceiving animate entities is time-sensitive in ways that pose significant evolutionary consequences. Participants completing a visual search task located evolutionarily relevant and animate objects fastest and with the least impact of high perceptual load. Evolutionarily novel and inanimate objects were located slowest and with the highest impact of perceptual load. Evolutionary relevance may importantly affect everyday visual information processing.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
40

Burgin, Mark, Eugene Eberbach und Rao Mikkilineni. „Processing Information in the Clouds“. Proceedings 47, Nr. 1 (07.05.2020): 25. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/proceedings2020047025.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
Annotation:
Cloud computing makes the necessary resources available to the appropriate computation to improve scaling, resiliency, and the efficiency of computations. This makes cloud computing a new paradigm for computation by upgrading its artificial intelligence (AI) to a higher order. To explore cloud computing using theoretical tools, we use cloud automata as a new model for computation. Higher-level AI requires infusing features of the human brain into AI systems such as incremental learning all the time. Consequently, we propose computational models that exhibit incremental learning without stopping (sentience). These features are inherent in reflexive Turing machines, inductive Turing machines, and limit Turing machines.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
41

Burgin, Mark, Eugene Eberbach und Rao Mikkilineni. „Processing Information in the Clouds“. Proceedings 47, Nr. 1 (07.05.2020): 25. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/proceedings47010025.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
Annotation:
Cloud computing makes the necessary resources available to the appropriate computation to improve scaling, resiliency, and the efficiency of computations. This makes cloud computing a new paradigm for computation by upgrading its artificial intelligence (AI) to a higher order. To explore cloud computing using theoretical tools, we use cloud automata as a new model for computation. Higher-level AI requires infusing features of the human brain into AI systems such as incremental learning all the time. Consequently, we propose computational models that exhibit incremental learning without stopping (sentience). These features are inherent in reflexive Turing machines, inductive Turing machines, and limit Turing machines.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
42

K.N., Sridevi, und Prakasha S. „Doc-To-Tokens based Pre-Processing in Information Retrieval System“. Webology 18, SI05 (30.10.2021): 570–79. http://dx.doi.org/10.14704/web/v18si05/web18247.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
43

Venturino, Michael. „Timesharing, Cognition, and Human Performance in Complex Information Systems“. Proceedings of the Human Factors Society Annual Meeting 30, Nr. 9 (September 1986): 880–81. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/154193128603000906.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
Annotation:
Applications of advanced technology to modern systems have resulted in the requirement for the human operator to process large volumes of information at rapid rates. Such systems may impose substantial attentional and timesharing requirements on the operator, often exceeding human information processing capabilities. Consequently, there is a critical need to investigate and understand the human information processing system in order to identify generalizable, empirically derived principles of human timesharing capabilities. Of particular interest are current theoretical approaches to human attention and information processing limitations that suggest ways to improve timesharing performance. Two of these approaches, automatic information processing theory and multiple resource theory are especially relevant to the timesharing problem.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
44

Massaro, Dominic W., und Donald A. Norman. „Memory and Attention: An Introduction to Human Information Processing“. American Journal of Psychology 107, Nr. 4 (1994): 597. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1423002.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
45

Hatamoto, K., M. Nagamachi, K. Ito und T. Tsuji. „Human visual information processing on formation of subjective contours“. Japanese journal of ergonomics 25, Supplement (1989): 112–13. http://dx.doi.org/10.5100/jje.25.supplement_112.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
46

Godlewska, Magdalena. „Smart Document-Centric Processing of Human Oriented Information Flows“. Computing and Informatics 37, Nr. 3 (2018): 673–92. http://dx.doi.org/10.4149/cai_2018_3_673.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
47

Rappelsberger, P., N. Vath, S. Weiss, E. Möller, G. Grießbach, H. Witte und B. Schack. „EEG Frequency and Phase Coupling during Human Information Processing“. Methods of Information in Medicine 40, Nr. 02 (2001): 106–11. http://dx.doi.org/10.1055/s-0038-1634470.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
Annotation:
AbstractNeuronal activity during information processing is represented by oscillations within local or widespread neuronal networks. These oscillations may be recorded by the EEG (electroencephalogram). The oscillatory interaction between neuronal ensembles may be at one single frequency or at different frequencies due to non-linear coupling. The investigation of momentary coherence and phase enables the examination of synchronized oscillatory network activity during fast-changing cognitive processes. On this basis information transfer from occipital areas towards frontal areas could be described during processing of visual presented words. Non-linear phase coupling between oscillations with different frequencies during memory processing was detected by means of cross-bicoherence.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
48

Foss, Donald J. „Review of Human Information Processing: Measures, Mechanisms, and Models.“ Contemporary Psychology: A Journal of Reviews 36, Nr. 3 (März 1991): 263. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/029570.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
49

Sobol, M. G., und G. Klein. „New graphics as computerized displays for human information processing“. IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics 19, Nr. 4 (1989): 893–98. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/21.35357.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
50

Strawser, Jerry R. „Human Information Processing the Consistency of Audit Risk Judgments“. Accounting and Business Research 21, Nr. 81 (Dezember 1990): 67–75. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00014788.1990.9729405.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
Wir bieten Rabatte auf alle Premium-Pläne für Autoren, deren Werke in thematische Literatursammlungen aufgenommen wurden. Kontaktieren Sie uns, um einen einzigartigen Promo-Code zu erhalten!

Zur Bibliographie