Zeitschriftenartikel zum Thema „Cognitive science“

Um die anderen Arten von Veröffentlichungen zu diesem Thema anzuzeigen, folgen Sie diesem Link: Cognitive science.

Geben Sie eine Quelle nach APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard und anderen Zitierweisen an

Wählen Sie eine Art der Quelle aus:

Machen Sie sich mit Top-50 Zeitschriftenartikel für die Forschung zum Thema "Cognitive science" bekannt.

Neben jedem Werk im Literaturverzeichnis ist die Option "Zur Bibliographie hinzufügen" verfügbar. Nutzen Sie sie, wird Ihre bibliographische Angabe des gewählten Werkes nach der nötigen Zitierweise (APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver usw.) automatisch gestaltet.

Sie können auch den vollen Text der wissenschaftlichen Publikation im PDF-Format herunterladen und eine Online-Annotation der Arbeit lesen, wenn die relevanten Parameter in den Metadaten verfügbar sind.

Sehen Sie die Zeitschriftenartikel für verschiedene Spezialgebieten durch und erstellen Sie Ihre Bibliographie auf korrekte Weise.

1

Alberto Greco. „Cognitive science and cognitive sciences“. Journal of Cognitive Science 13, Nr. 4 (Dezember 2012): 471–85. http://dx.doi.org/10.17791/jcs.2012.13.4.471.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
2

Favela, Luis H., und Jonathan Martin. „“Cognition” and Dynamical Cognitive Science“. Minds and Machines 27, Nr. 2 (07.12.2016): 331–55. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11023-016-9411-4.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
3

Ross, Don. „Economics, cognitive science and social cognition“. Cognitive Systems Research 9, Nr. 1-2 (März 2008): 125–35. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogsys.2007.06.010.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
4

Sofronova, Lidia. „Historical Cognition and Cognitive Sciences: New in Russian Historiography“. ISTORIYA 12, Nr. 8 (106) (2021): 0. http://dx.doi.org/10.18254/s207987840016952-6.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
Annotation:
The article presents an analytical review of the recent literature on cognitive history, especially the Russian collective monograph “Cognitive Sciences and Historical Cognition”, published in 2020. It traces the patterns typical for interdisciplinary research not only within the humanitarian disciplines, but also at the “borders” between the humanities and the “natural sciences”. The article highlights the paradoxical and productive nature of the “mutual interventions” of cognitive science and the humanities, which contribute to overcoming “atomism” both within the humanities and at the “frontier” between them and the natural science disciplines.
5

Boring, Ronald Laurids. „Cognitive science“. XRDS: Crossroads, The ACM Magazine for Students 10, Nr. 2 (Dezember 2003): 1. http://dx.doi.org/10.1145/1027328.1027329.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
6

Wimer Brakel, Linda A. „Cognitive Science“. Psychoanalytic Quarterly 64, Nr. 2 (April 1995): 417–28. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/21674086.1995.11927458.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
7

Dartnall, Terry, Steve Torrance, Mark Coulson, Stephen Nunn, Brendan Kitts, R. F. Port, T. van Gelder, Donald Peterson und Philip Gerrans. „Cognitive science“. Metascience 5, Nr. 1 (März 1996): 95–166. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/bf02988881.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
8

Stern, Elsbeth. „Cognitive Psychology and Cognitive Science“. Contemporary Psychology: A Journal of Reviews 36, Nr. 6 (Juni 1991): 485–86. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/029811.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
9

Leite, Diego Azevedo. „Neo-mechanistic explanatory integration for cognitive science: the problem of reduction remains“. Sofia 8, Nr. 1 (05.09.2019): 124–45. http://dx.doi.org/10.47456/sofia.v8i1.23198.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
Annotation:
One of the central aims of the neo-mechanistic framework for the neural and cognitive sciences is to construct a pluralistic integration of scientific explanations, allowing for a weak explanatory autonomy of higher-level sciences, such as cognitive science. This integration involves understanding human cognition as information processing occurring in multi-level human neuro-cognitive mechanisms, explained by multi-level neuro-cognitive models. Strong explanatory neuro-cognitive reduction, however, poses a significant challenge to this pluralist ambition and the weak autonomy of cognitive science derived therefrom. Based on research in current molecular and cellular neuroscience, the framework holds that the best strategy for integrating human neuro-cognitive theories is through direct reductive explanations based on molecular and cellular neural processes. It is my aim to investigate whether the neo-mechanistic framework can meet the challenge. I argue that leading neo-mechanists offer some significant replies; however, they are not able yet to completely remove strong explanatory reductionism from their own framework.
10

Bradley, Dianne. „Cognitive science and the language/ cognition distinction“. Aphasiology 3, Nr. 8 (Dezember 1989): 755–57. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02687038908249045.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
11

Wang, Yingxu, Bernard Carlos Widrow, Bo Zhang, Witold Kinsner, Kenji Sugawara, Fuchun Sun, Jianhua Lu, Thomas Weise und Du Zhang. „Perspectives on the Field of Cognitive Informatics and its Future Development“. International Journal of Cognitive Informatics and Natural Intelligence 5, Nr. 1 (Januar 2011): 1–17. http://dx.doi.org/10.4018/jcini.2011010101.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
Annotation:
The contemporary wonder of sciences and engineering has recently refocused on the beginning point of: how the brain processes internal and external information autonomously and cognitively rather than imperatively like conventional computers. Cognitive Informatics (CI) is a transdisciplinary enquiry of computer science, information sciences, cognitive science, and intelligence science that investigates the internal information processing mechanisms and processes of the brain and natural intelligence, as well as their engineering applications in cognitive computing. This paper reports a set of eight position statements presented in the plenary panel of IEEE ICCI’10 on Cognitive Informatics and Its Future Development contributed from invited panelists who are part of the world’s renowned researchers and scholars in the field of cognitive informatics and cognitive computing.
12

Wang, Yingxu. „On the Mathematical Theories and Cognitive Foundations of Information“. International Journal of Cognitive Informatics and Natural Intelligence 9, Nr. 3 (Juli 2015): 42–64. http://dx.doi.org/10.4018/ijcini.2015070103.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
Annotation:
A recent discovery in computer and software sciences is that information in general is a deterministic abstract quantity rather than a probability-based property of the nature. Information is a general form of abstract objects represented by symbolical, mathematical, communication, computing, and cognitive systems. Therefore, information science is one of the contemporary scientific disciplines collectively known as abstract sciences such as system, information, cybernetics, cognition, knowledge, and intelligence sciences. This paper presents the cognitive foundations, mathematical models, and formal properties of information towards an extended theory of information science. From this point of view, information is classified into the categories of classic, computational, and cognitive information in the contexts of communication, computation, and cognition, respectively. Based on the three generations of information theories, a coherent framework of contemporary information is introduced, which reveals the nature of information and the fundamental principles of information science and engineering.
13

WANNER, E. „A Science of Cognition: Method and Tactics in Cognitive Science.“ Science 228, Nr. 4695 (05.04.1985): 68. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.228.4695.68.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
14

Wang, Yingxu, George Baciu, Yiyu Yao, Witold Kinsner, Keith Chan, Bo Zhang, Stuart Hameroff et al. „Perspectives on Cognitive Informatics and Cognitive Computing“. International Journal of Cognitive Informatics and Natural Intelligence 4, Nr. 1 (Januar 2010): 1–29. http://dx.doi.org/10.4018/jcini.2010010101.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
Annotation:
Cognitive informatics is a transdisciplinary enquiry of computer science, information sciences, cognitive science, and intelligence science that investigates the internal information processing mechanisms and processes of the brain and natural intelligence, as well as their engineering applications in cognitive computing. Cognitive computing is an emerging paradigm of intelligent computing methodologies and systems based on cognitive informatics that implements computational intelligence by autonomous inferences and perceptions mimicking the mechanisms of the brain. This article presents a set of collective perspectives on cognitive informatics and cognitive computing, as well as their applications in abstract intelligence, computational intelligence, computational linguistics, knowledge representation, symbiotic computing, granular computing, semantic computing, machine learning, and social computing.
15

Boster, James S. „Cognitive Anthropology Is a Cognitive Science“. Topics in Cognitive Science 4, Nr. 3 (18.06.2012): 372–78. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1756-8765.2012.01204.x.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
16

Kozhevnikova, Larisa A. „Librarianship as Knowledge and Science“. Bibliotekovedenie [Russian Journal of Library Science], Nr. 1 (24.02.2011): 22–27. http://dx.doi.org/10.25281/0869-608x-2011-0-1-22-27.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
Annotation:
The paper deals with cognitive structure of library science. A cognitive determination of various types of library knowledge is analyzed. Knowledge of institutional character and scientific knowledge are determined. The author examines essential characteristics of scientific knowledge, its objective, and stages of cognition and cognitive values of library science.
17

Carey, Susan. „Cognitive science and science education.“ American Psychologist 41, Nr. 10 (1986): 1123–30. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066x.41.10.1123.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
18

Stewart, James. „Cognitive science and science education“. European Journal of Science Education 7, Nr. 1 (Januar 1985): 1–17. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/0140528850070101.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
19

Kalia, Lipika Priyadarshini. „Computer Science is Cognitive Science“. Advanced Science Letters 22, Nr. 2 (01.02.2016): 538–39. http://dx.doi.org/10.1166/asl.2016.6851.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
20

Gabrić, Petar. „Konferencija: Cognitive Science“. Drustvena istrazivanja 28, Nr. 2 (10.06.2019): 361–62. http://dx.doi.org/10.5559/di.28.2.11.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
21

Hovhannisyan, Garri. „Humanistic cognitive science.“ Humanistic Psychologist 46, Nr. 1 (März 2018): 30–52. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/hum0000074.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
22

Carey, Susan. „Thescienceof cognitive science“. Social Anthropology 23, Nr. 2 (Mai 2015): 204–7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1469-8676.12119.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
23

Dean, Michael P. „Review: Cognitive Science“. Perception 29, Nr. 6 (Juni 2000): 755–56. http://dx.doi.org/10.1068/p2906rvw.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
24

Rönnberg, Jerker, Mary Rudner und Thomas Lunner. „Cognitive Hearing Science“. Trends in Amplification 15, Nr. 3 (22.05.2011): 140–48. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1084713811409762.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
25

Johansen-Berg, Heidi. „Cognitive science fiction“. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 5, Nr. 6 (Juni 2001): 233. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01694-6.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
26

WALTZ, D. „Cognitive science news“. Cognitive Science 9, Nr. 4 (Dezember 1985): ii—iv. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0364-0213(85)80017-3.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
27

Goldstone, Robert L. „Becoming Cognitive Science“. Topics in Cognitive Science 11, Nr. 4 (Oktober 2019): 902–13. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/tops.12463.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
28

Carr, Thomas H. „Understanding Cognitive Science?“ Contemporary Psychology: A Journal of Reviews 35, Nr. 11 (November 1990): 1079–80. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/030592.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
29

DANZIGER, EVE. „Cognitive Dimensions of Social Science.:Cognitive Dimensions of Social Science“. Journal of Linguistic Anthropology 14, Nr. 2 (Dezember 2004): 290–92. http://dx.doi.org/10.1525/jlin.2004.14.2.290.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
30

Motz, Benjamin. „Cognitive science in popular film: the Cognitive Science Movie Index“. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 17, Nr. 10 (Oktober 2013): 483–85. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.08.002.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
31

Sedlacek, Miroslav. „Models of Mind That Are Implied by Cognitive Science“. JOURNAL OF INTERNATIONAL BUSINESS RESEARCH AND MARKETING 2, Nr. 5 (2017): 33–36. http://dx.doi.org/10.18775/jibrm.1849-8558.2015.25.3005.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
Annotation:
Three models of mind were proposed during the history of cognitive science: functionalist, psychological, and neural model. The functionalist model of mind is based on symbol manipulation (computation). This model has two versions: the conception of mind as an innate information processor and problem solver, and mind as innate cognitive and visual system. A psychological model of mind is the extension by “common sense”. The alternative is mind as a cognitive and visual system that is based mostly on manipulation of internal mental data. The neural model is the only model of embodied mind. It has two versions: non-pragmatic and pragmatic (autopoietic, distributed, and social mind).
32

Asma, Stephen. „Adaptive Imagination: Toward a Mythopoetic Cognitive Science“. Evolutionary Studies in Imaginative Culture 5, Nr. 2 (01.12.2021): 1–32. http://dx.doi.org/10.26613/esic.5.2.236.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
Annotation:
Abstract A mythopoetic paradigm or perspective sees the world primarily as a dramatic story of competing personal intentions, rather than a system of objective impersonal laws. Asma (2017) argued that our contemporary imaginative cognition is evolutionarily conserved-it has structural and functional similarities to premodern Homo sapiens’s cognition. This article will (i) outline the essential features of mythopoetic cognition or adaptive imagination, (ii) delineate the adaptive sociocultural advantages of mythopoetic cognition, (iii) explain the phylogenetic and ontogenetic mechanisms that give rise to human mythopoetic mind (i.e., genetically endowed simulation and associational systems that underwrite diverse symbolic systems), (iv) show how mythopoetic cognition challeng­es contemporary trends in cognitive science and philosophy, and (v) recognize and outline empirical approaches for a new cognitive science of the imagination.
33

Castro, Nichol, und Cynthia S. Q. Siew. „Contributions of modern network science to the cognitive sciences: revisiting research spirals of representation and process“. Proceedings of the Royal Society A: Mathematical, Physical and Engineering Sciences 476, Nr. 2238 (Juni 2020): 20190825. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspa.2019.0825.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
Annotation:
Modelling the structure of cognitive systems is a central goal of the cognitive sciences—a goal that has greatly benefitted from the application of network science approaches. This paper provides an overview of how network science has been applied to the cognitive sciences, with a specific focus on the two research ‘spirals’ of cognitive sciences related to the representation and processes of the human mind. For each spiral, we first review classic papers in the psychological sciences that have drawn on graph-theoretic ideas or frameworks before the advent of modern network science approaches. We then discuss how current research in these areas has been shaped by modern network science, which provides the mathematical framework and methodological tools for psychologists to (i) represent cognitive network structure and (ii) investigate and model the psychological processes that occur in these cognitive networks. Finally, we briefly comment on the future of, and the challenges facing, cognitive network science.
34

Chokr, Nader N. „Mind, consciousness, and cognition: Phenomenology vs. cognitive science“. Husserl Studies 9, Nr. 3 (1992): 179–97. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/bf00142815.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
35

Siew, Cynthia S. Q., Dirk U. Wulff, Nicole M. Beckage, Yoed N. Kenett und Ana Meštrović. „Cognitive Network Science: A Review of Research on Cognition through the Lens of Network Representations, Processes, and Dynamics“. Complexity 2019 (17.06.2019): 1–24. http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2019/2108423.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
Annotation:
Network science provides a set of quantitative methods to investigate complex systems, including human cognition. Although cognitive theories in different domains are strongly based on a network perspective, the application of network science methodologies to quantitatively study cognition has so far been limited in scope. This review demonstrates how network science approaches have been applied to the study of human cognition and how network science can uniquely address and provide novel insight on important questions related to the complexity of cognitive systems and the processes that occur within those systems. Drawing on the literature in cognitive network science, with a focus on semantic and lexical networks, we argue three key points. (i) Network science provides a powerful quantitative approach to represent cognitive systems. (ii) The network science approach enables cognitive scientists to achieve a deeper understanding of human cognition by capturing how the structure, i.e., the underlying network, and processes operating on a network structure interact to produce behavioral phenomena. (iii) Network science provides a quantitative framework to model the dynamics of cognitive systems, operationalized as structural changes in cognitive systems on different timescales and resolutions. Finally, we highlight key milestones that the field of cognitive network science needs to achieve as it matures in order to provide continued insights into the nature of cognitive structures and processes.
36

Powell, Janet M., Earl Hunt und Mary Pepping. „Collaboration Between Cognitive Science and Cognitive Rehabilitation“. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation 19, Nr. 3 (2004): 266–76. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00001199-200405000-00007.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
37

Chater, Nick. „Can cognitive science create a cognitive economics?“ Cognition 135 (Februar 2015): 52–55. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.015.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
38

Crippen, Matthew. „Embodied Cognition and Perception: Dewey, Science and Skepticism“. Contemporary Pragmatism 14, Nr. 1 (30.05.2017): 112–34. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/18758185-01401007.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
Annotation:
This article examines how Modern theories of mind remain even in some materialistic and hence ontologically anti-dualistic views; and shows how Dewey, anticipating Merleau-Ponty and 4E cognitive scientists, repudiates these theories. Throughout I place Dewey’s thought in the context of scientific inquiry, both recent and historical and including the cognitive as well as traditional sciences; and I show how he incorporated sciences of his day into his thought, while also anticipating enactive cognitive science. While emphasizing Dewey’s continued relevance, my main goal is to show how his scientifically informed account of perception and cognition combats skepticism propagated by certain scientific visions, exacerbated by commonplace notions about mind, that jointly suggest that human beings lack genuine access to reality.
39

Elin, Mirza Niaz Zaman. „Exploring the Quantum Nature of Human Cognition: Integrating Quantum Cognitive Science and Advanced Cognitive Assessment through the Verbovisual Cognitive Function Screening Tool (VCFST)“. International Journal of Interdisciplinary Research Methods 10, Nr. 2 (15.02.2023): 49–63. http://dx.doi.org/10.37745/ijirm.14/vol10n24963.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
Annotation:
The Verbovisual Cognitive Function Screening Tool (VCFST) is a cognitive screening tool that aims to evaluate cognitive abilities by drawing upon established theories such as dual-coding theory, common coding theory, propositional theory, and ideomotor theory. However, recent developments in quantum cognitive science offer promising applications to enhance the assessment model. This article explores the utilization of quantum cognition concepts within the VCFST and discusses its implications for cognitive assessment. Quantum cognitive science is an emerging interdisciplinary field that applies principles from quantum physics to the study of human cognition. It suggests that cognitive processes may not adhere strictly to classical logic and linear thinking but can exhibit characteristics of quantum phenomena such as superposition, entanglement, and contextuality. By incorporating quantum concepts into cognitive assessment, a more nuanced understanding of complex cognitive processes can be achieved.The VCFST, with its focus on verbovisual factors and comprehensive assessment of cognitive domains, can benefit from the integration of quantum cognitive science principles. Superposition, borrowed from quantum physics, implies that individuals can simultaneously perceive and attend to multiple stimuli. By considering the superposition of verbovisual stimuli within the VCFST, a more accurate representation of cognitive abilities can be obtained, capturing the simultaneous processing of different types of information. Entanglement, another key principle of quantum cognition, can be applied to evaluate the interconnectedness of cognitive domains within the VCFST. Analogous to entangled particles, changes in one cognitive domain may influence others. By assessing the impact of changes in one domain on performance in other domains, the VCFST can provide a holistic understanding of cognitive functioning. Contextuality, a fundamental concept in quantum cognition, emphasizes the role of context in shaping cognitive processes. By incorporating contextuality into the VCFST, contextual cues can be introduced, and stimulus presentation can be varied to reflect real-world cognitive demands. This approach enables a more ecologically valid measure of cognitive abilities, considering the influence of context on cognitive processing. Embracing quantum cognitive science principles in the VCFST offers several advantages, including capturing the complexity of cognitive processes, accounting for non-linear dynamics, and exploring potential quantum-like effects within human cognition. Future research can further refine the VCFST by conducting quantitative studies to assess the efficacy of incorporating quantum concepts into cognitive assessment and developing computational models inspired by quantum cognitive science.The utilization of quantum cognitive science concepts within the VCFST shows promise for advancing cognitive assessment practices. By incorporating principles such as superposition, entanglement, and contextuality, the VCFST can provide a comprehensive and nuanced evaluation of cognitive abilities. Further research in quantum cognitive science and the refinement of assessment methodologies can contribute to the development of more advanced and accurate cognitive assessment tools, facilitating personalized diagnostic and therapeutic strategies for cognitive disorders and neurodegenerative conditions. The integration of quantum cognitive science into cognitive assessment represents an exciting frontier that can revolutionize our understanding of human cognition.
40

Maltseva, Kateryna. „Bridging sociology with anthropology and cognitive science perspectives to assess shared cultural knowledge“. Sociology: Theory, Methods, Marketing, stmm 2020 (1) (16.03.2020): 108–18. http://dx.doi.org/10.15407/sociology2020.01.108.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
Annotation:
Following the cognitive revolution of the 1960s, cultural variation in behavior and knowledge has been a long-standing subject in social sciences. The “cognitive turn” in sociology brought to light many interesting issues and complex questions. The present publication addresses both theoretical and — to some extent — methodological challenges faced by the sociologists engaged in researching shared cultural variation within the culture-and-cognition research agenda, and compares it with the status quo in cousin social sciences that share the same cognitive perspective on culture. I specifically focus on the conceptual junctures that follow from the assumptions of shared cultural knowledge and intersubjectively shared cultural worldviews to highlight the important features of culture which can be effectively used for quantitative assessment of complex cultural processes. While I discuss various aspects of the findings and failings attributable to the culture-and-cognition research direction, my principal concern centers on encouraging more enhanced and sensitized interdisciplinary communication, as well as maximized intersections between cognitively oriented studies of culture in different social sciences, to bring the sociological studies of culture and cognition to full fruition.
41

Adey, Philip. „Science education research and cognitive science“. Research in Science Education 25, Nr. 1 (März 1995): 101–13. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/bf02356463.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
42

Busemeyer, Jerome R. „Cognitive science contributions to decision science“. Cognition 135 (Februar 2015): 43–46. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2014.11.010.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
43

Nalini, V. Hema. „Cognitive Science Perspective: Synectics As a Model of Learning Metaphors“. International Journal of Scientific Research 2, Nr. 6 (01.06.2012): 147–48. http://dx.doi.org/10.15373/22778179/june2013/47.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
44

Wang, Yingxu, Robert C. Berwick, Simon Haykin, Witold Pedrycz, Witold Kinsner, George Baciu, Du Zhang und C. Bhavsar. „Cognitive Informatics and Cognitive Computing in Year 10 and Beyond“. International Journal of Cognitive Informatics and Natural Intelligence 5, Nr. 4 (Oktober 2011): 1–21. http://dx.doi.org/10.4018/jcini.2011100101.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
Annotation:
Cognitive Informatics (CI) is a transdisciplinary enquiry of computer science, information sciences, cognitive science, and intelligence science that investigates into the internal information processing mechanisms and processes of the brain and natural intelligence, as well as their engineering applications in cognitive computing. The latest advances in CI leads to the establishment of cognitive computing theories and methodologies, as well as the development of Cognitive Computers (CogC) that perceive, infer, and learn. This paper reports a set of nine position statements presented in the plenary panel of IEEE ICCI*CC’11 on Cognitive Informatics in Year 10 and Beyond contributed from invited panelists who are part of the world’s renowned researchers and scholars in the field of cognitive informatics and cognitive computing.
45

Дудуш, А. С., І. І. Сачук, Сальман Оваід und А. К. Бідун. „Science & technology trends in cognitive radar concept“. Системи обробки інформації, Nr. 3(166) (24.09.2021): 22–34. http://dx.doi.org/10.30748/soi.2021.166.02.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
Annotation:
Currently, human operators provide cognition in a radar system. However, advances in the “digitization” of radar front-ends, including digital arbitrary waveform generators (AWG) and advanced high performance embedded computing (HPEC) make it possible to vary all key radar parameters (power, pulse length, number of pulses, pulse repetition frequency (PRF), modulation, frequency, polarization) on a pulse-by-pulse basis within ns or ms and over a wide operating range. This timescale is much faster than the decision-making ability of a human operator. The cognitive-inspired techniques in radar, that are intensively developing last years, mimic elements of human cognition and the use of external knowledge to use the available system resources in an optimal way for the current goal and environment. Radar systems based on the perception-action cycle of cognition that senses the environment, learns relevant information from it about the target and the background and then adapts the radar to optimally satisfy the needs of the mission according to a desired goal are called cognitive radars. In the article, recent ideas and applications of cognitive radars were analyzed.
46

Morais, José. „Applied Psycholinguistics: A Science at the crossroads of cognition and language“. Signo 47, Nr. 88 (03.01.2022): 96–112. http://dx.doi.org/10.17058/signo.v47i88.17387.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
Annotation:
Abstract: Applied Psycholinguistics is a science that engages many others: experimental psychology, cognitive and neurocognitive sciences, linguistics, psychology of language and literacy, and educational and remediation sciences. The present paper’s objective is to show Science is itself a changing combination of ever-changing sciences without close boundaries, which implies the necessity of crossing domains in both research and learning. After a reminder of several topics of relevance to applied psycholinguistics, which concern mental processing, how cognition relates to the brain and to language, and how cognition and language engendered literacy, I argue that research in the corresponding sciences needs to be opened to other dimensions, such as society, culture, and politics. Finally, I evoke the history of the ideas regarding the isolationism of individualized sciences vs. their unification, taking, as examples of the latter, the early Marxism, and the International Movement for the Unity of Science from the fourth decade of the 20th century. Keywords: Applied Psycholinguistics; literacy as product of cognition and language; concept of Science; history of scientific ideas; permeability of science to culture and politics.
47

Gibbs, Raymond W., und Noel E. Sharkey. „Advances in Cognitive Science“. American Journal of Psychology 101, Nr. 4 (1988): 577. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1423234.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
48

Rescorla, Michael. „Representation in Cognitive Science“. Philosophical Review 130, Nr. 1 (01.01.2021): 180–85. http://dx.doi.org/10.1215/00318108-8699643.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
49

Chernikova, Irina V., und Yuliya V. Loginovskaya. „Consciousness in Cognitive Science“. Vestnik Tomskogo gosudarstvennogo universiteta. Filosofiya, sotsiologiya, politologiya, Nr. 53 (01.02.2020): 32–42. http://dx.doi.org/10.17223/1998863x/53/4.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
50

Tauzin, Tibor Miklós. „Definitions in cognitive science“. Magyar Pszichológiai Szemle 64, Nr. 4 (01.12.2009): 729–51. http://dx.doi.org/10.1556/mpszle.64.2009.4.6.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
Annotation:
A megfelelő definíció és definíciós metódus rövid meghatározása után a kognitív tudományban használt definíciók jellemző hibái kerülnek bemutatásra. A definíciótípusok, a gyakori definíciós hibák és több diszciplína definíciópéldáinak áttekintése után feltárjuk a kognitív tudományban uralkodó definíciós gyakorlatot. A nem megfelelő definíciók által okozott következmények elemzését követően a megfelelő definíciók létrehozásának módját és ennek pozitív hatásait mutatjuk be.

Zur Bibliographie